[1]
|
Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar. Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 3, 117-127.
doi:10.1093/wsr/wsr014
|
[2]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (1997). Reading in Arabic orthography: The effects of vowels and context on reading accuracy of poor and skilled Arabic native readers in reading paragraphs, sentences, and isolated words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 465-482.
doi:10.1023/A:1025034220924
|
[3]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (1996). The role of vowels and context in reading of highly skilled native Arabic readers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 629-641. doi:10.1007/BF01712413
|
[4]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (1998). Reading Arabic texts: Effects of text type, reader type and vowelization. Reading and Writing, 10, 105-119.
doi:10.1023/A:1007906222227
|
[5]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading Semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing, 14, 39-59.
doi:10.1023/A:1008147606320
|
[6]
|
Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16, 423-442.
doi:10.1023/A:1024237415143
|
[7]
|
Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (1995). Different orthographies, different context effects: The effects of Arabic sentence context in skilled and poor readers. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 16, 1-19. doi:10.1080/0270271950160101
|
[8]
|
Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621-635. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000316
|
[9]
|
Azzam, R. (1989). In the linguistics of literacy, orthography and reading of the Arabic language. In J. Aaron, & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and Writing Disorders in Different Orthographic Systems (pp. 203-218). Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-1041-6_12
|
[10]
|
Azzam, R. (1993). The nature of Arabic reading and spelling errors of young children. Reading and Writing, 5, 355-385.
doi:10.1007/BF01043112
|
[11]
|
Barker, A., Torgesen, K., & Wagner, K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334-345. doi:10.2307/747673
|
[12]
|
Bentin, S., & Ibrahim, R. (1996). New evidence for phonological processing during visual word recognition: The case of Arabic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 309-323. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.309
|
[13]
|
Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1987). Processing lexical ambiguity and visual word recognition in a deep orthography. Memory & Cognition, 15, 13-23. doi:10.3758/BF03197708
|
[14]
|
Berman, R. A. (1978). Modern Hebrew structure. Tel Aviv: University Publisher.
|
[15]
|
Brown, G. D. A., & Deavers, R. P. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 208-242.
doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2502
|
[16]
|
Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In: G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of Information Processing (pp. 151 216). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
|
[17]
|
Coltheart, M., Besner, D., Jonasson, J. T., & Davelaar, E. (1979). Phonological recording in the lexical decision task. Quarterly Journal of experimental Psychology, 31, 489-507.
doi:10.1080/14640747908400741
|
[18]
|
Corcos, E., & Willows, D. M. (1993). The processing of orthographic information. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual Processes in Reading and Reading Disabilities (pp. 163-188). Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
|
[19]
|
Eviatar, Z., Ibrahim, R., & Ganayim, D. (2004). Orthography and the hemispheres: Visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neuropsychology, 18, 174-184. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.174
|
[20]
|
Fender, M. (2008). Spelling knowledge and reading development: Insights from Arab ESL learners. Reading in a Foreign Language, 20, 19-42.
|
[21]
|
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. patterson, J. Marshall, & M. Clotheart (Eds.), Surface Dyslexia (pp. 301-330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
|
[22]
|
Frost, R. (1994). Prelexical and postlexical strategies in reading: Evidence from deep and shallow orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 116-129.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.1.116
|
[23]
|
Frost, R. (1995). Phonological computation and missing vowels: Mapping lexical involvement in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 398-408.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.2.398
|
[24]
|
Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71-99. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.71
|
[25]
|
Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic systems and skilled word recognition processes in reading. In M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The Science of Reading: A Handbook (pp. 272-295). Oxford: Blackwell.
doi:10.1002/9780470757642.ch15
|
[26]
|
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthogrsphical depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104-114. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.104
|
[27]
|
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 241-258. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_3
|
[28]
|
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. (2000). Orthographic factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 1-30. doi:10.1023/A:1008017710115
|
[29]
|
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2001). Children’s orthographic representation in English and Greek. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 3, 273-292.
|
[30]
|
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235-247.
doi:10.1017/S0142716403000134
|
[31]
|
Goswami, U., & Ziegler, J. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, development dyslexia, and skilled reading languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
|
[32]
|
Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of Arabic orthography slows its processing. Neurosychology, 16, 322-326. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.322
|
[33]
|
Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost, & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, Phonology, Morphology and Meaning (pp. 67-84). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62789-2
|
[34]
|
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315-334. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X
|
[35]
|
Leong, C. K., & Joshi, R. M. (1997). Cross-language studies of learning to read and spell: Phonologic and orthographic processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-1197-5
|
[36]
|
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
|
[37]
|
Roman, G., & Pavard, B. (1987). A comparative study: How we read Arabic and French. In J. K. O’Regan, & A. Levy-Schoen (Eds.), Eye movements: From physiology to cognition (pp. 431-440). Amsterdam: North Holland Elsevier.
|
[38]
|
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 431-451. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000225
|
[39]
|
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21, 481-504.
doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x
|
[40]
|
Shankweiler, D. (1989). How problems of comprehension are related to difficulties in word reading. In D. Shankweiler, & I. Y. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology and Reading Disability: Solving the Reading Puzzle (pp. 35-68). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
|
[41]
|
Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris, & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to Read and Write: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective (pp. 98-111). New York: Cambridge University Press.
|
[42]
|
Shatil, E., & Share, D. (2003). Cognitive antecedents of early reading ability: A test of the modularity hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 1-31. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00106-1
|
[43]
|
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of Current Reading Research and Practice: The Perils of Overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584-615.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
|
[44]
|
Shimron, J. (1999). The role of vowel signs in Hebrew: Beyond word recognition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 301-319. doi:10.1023/A:1008045316692
|
[45]
|
Shimron, J., & Sivan, T. (1994). Reading proficiency and orthography: Evidence from Hebrew and English. Language Learning, 44, 5-27.
|
[46]
|
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia, second edition. Oxford: Blackwell.
|
[47]
|
Seidenberg, M. S., & Zevin, J. D. (2006). Connectionist models in developmental cognitive neuroscience: Critical periods and the paradox of success. In Y. Munakata, & M. Johnson (Eds.), Attention & Performance XXI: Processes of Change in Brain and Cognitive Development (pp. 585-612). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
|
[48]
|
Stanovich, K. E., & Stanovich, P. (1999). How research might inform the debate about early reading acquisition. In J. V. Oakhill, & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading Development and the Teaching of Reading (13-31). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
|
[49]
|
Taouk, M., & Coltheart, M. (2004). The cognitive processes involved in learning to read in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 27-57.
doi:10.1023/B:READ.0000013831.91795.ec
|
[50]
|
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 124, 107-136. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.107
|
[51]
|
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Development Psychology, 30, 73-87. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.73
|
[52]
|
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
|