[1]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (2000). Effects of Exposure to Literary Arabic on Reading Comprehension in a Diglossic Situation. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 147-157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008133701024
|
[2]
|
Ayari, S. (1996). Diglossia and Illiteracy in the Arab World. Language and Culture and Curriculum, 9, 243-252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07908319609525233
|
[3]
|
Azzam, R. (1993). The Nature of Arabic Reading and Spelling Errors of Young Children. Reading and Writing, 5, 355-385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01043112
|
[4]
|
Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the Double Deficit Hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 69-88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-997-0021-y
|
[5]
|
Bowers, P. G., & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming Speed Deficits in Reading Disability: Multiple Measures if a Singular Process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195-219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(91)90032-N
|
[6]
|
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical Links among Naming Speed, Precise Timing Mechanisms, and Orthographic Skill in Dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01026919
|
[7]
|
Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in Reading: Synchronization of Processes. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
|
[8]
|
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid Automatized Naming of Pictured Objects, Colors, Letters and Numbers by Normal Children. Cortex, 10, 186-202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(74)80009-2
|
[9]
|
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976a). Naming of Objects by Dyslexic and Other Learning Disabled Children. Brain and language, 3, 1-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(76)90001-8
|
[10]
|
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976b). Rapid Automatized Naming (R.A.N): Dyslexia Differentiated from Other Learning Disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471-479. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0
|
[11]
|
Escribano, C. L., & Katzir, T. (2008). Are Phonological Processes Separate from Naming Speed in a Shallow Orthography? Education and Psychology, 6, 641-666.
|
[12]
|
Everatt, J., & Zabell, C. (2002). Surface and Phonological Subtypes of Adult Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8, 160-177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.223
|
[13]
|
Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The Evidence for Temporal Processing Deficit Linked to Dyslexia: A Review. Psychonomic Society, 2, 460-493. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210983
|
[14]
|
Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 14, 47-56.
|
[15]
|
Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic Systems and Skilled Word Recognition Processes in Reading. In M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The Science of Reading: A Handbook (pp. 272-295). Oxford: Blackwell. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch15
|
[16]
|
Geshwind, N. (1972). Language and the Brain. Scientific American, 226, 76-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0472-76
|
[17]
|
Greene, B. A., & Royer, J. M. (1994). A Developmental Review of Response Time Data That Support a Cognitive Components Model of Reading. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 141-172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02208971
|
[18]
|
Ibrahim, R. (2009). The Cognitive Basis of Diglossia in Arabic: Evidence from Repetition Priming Study within and between Languages. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 12, 95-105.
|
[19]
|
Jimenez, E. J., Hernandez, V. I., Rodriguez, C., Guzman, R., Diaz, A., & Ortiz, R. (2008). The Double-Deficit Hypothesis in Spanish Developmental Dyslexia. Top Lang Disorders, 28, 46-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.adt.0000311415.69966.76
|
[20]
|
Katzir, T., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). Reading Fluency: The Whole Is More than the Parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 51-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5
|
[21]
|
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). Defining Dyslexia, Comorbidity, Teacher’s Knowledge of Language and Reading: A Definition of Dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9
|
[22]
|
Meyer, M. S., & Felton, R. H. (1999). Repeated Reading to Enhance Fluency: Old Approaches and New Direction. Annuals of Dyslexia, 49, 283-306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-999-0027-8
|
[23]
|
Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A., & Felton, R. H. (1998). Longitudinal Course of Rapid Naming in Disabled and Nondisabled Readers. Annuals of Dyslexia, 48, 91-114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-998-0005-6
|
[24]
|
Perfetti, C. A. (1977). Language Comprehension and Fast Decoding: Some Psycholinguistic Prerequisites for Skilled Reading Comprehension. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Cognition, Curriculum and Comprehension (pp. 20-41). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
|
[25]
|
Perfetti, C. A., Finger, E., & Hogaboam, T. W. (1978). Sources of Vocalization Latency Differences between Skilled and Less Skilled Young Readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 730-739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.70.5.730
|
[26]
|
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of Reading Fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and Orthographic Factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 559-582. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3180-4
|
[27]
|
Scarborough, H. S. (1998a). Early Identification of Children at Risk for Reading Disabilities: Phonological Awareness and Some Other Promising Predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific Reading Disability: A View of the Spectrum (pp. 75-119). Timonium, MD: York Press.
|
[28]
|
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation Literacy Acquisition in European Orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859
|
[29]
|
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of Current Reading Research Practice: The Perils of Over-Reliance on an “Outlier” Orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584-615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
|
[30]
|
Share, D. L., & Leiken, M. (2004). Language Impairment at School Entry and Later Reading Disability: Connections at Lexical versus Supralexical Levels of Reading. Scientific studies of Reading, 8, 87-110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_5
|
[31]
|
Smythe, I., Everatt, J., & Salter, R. (Eds.) (2004). The International Book of Dyslexia. London: Wiley.
|
[32]
|
Swanson, L. B. (1989). Analyzing Naming Speed-Reading Relationships in Children. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Waterloo, Ontario: University of Waterloo.
|
[33]
|
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W., & Merzenich, M. (1996). Language Comprehension in Language-Learning Impaired Children Improved with Acoustically Modified Speech. Science, 271, 81-84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.271.5245.81
|
[34]
|
Van den Bos, K. P. (1998). IQ, Phonological Awareness and Continuous-Naming Speed Related to Dutch Poor Decoding Children’s Performance on Two Word Identification Tests. Dyslexia, 4, 73-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(199806)4:2<73::AID-DYS104>3.0.CO;2-#
|
[35]
|
Van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J. H., & Spelberg, H. C. (2002). Life Span Data on Continuous Naming Speeds of Numbers, Letters, Colors, and Pictured Objects, and Word Reading Speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 25-49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0601_02
|
[36]
|
Wagner, R. K., & Torgensen, J. K. (1987). The Nature of Phonological Processing and Its Causal Role in the Acquisition of Reading Skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192
|
[37]
|
Wagner, R. K., Laughon, P. L., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of Young Readers’ Phonological Processing Abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83-103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.1.83
|
[38]
|
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of Developmental Dyslexia in a Regular Writing System. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010122
|
[39]
|
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The Double-Deficit Hypothesis and Difficulties in Learning to Read a Regular Orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668-680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668
|
[40]
|
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The Question of Naming-Speed Deficits in Developmental Reading Disabilities: An Introduction to the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 19, 1-24.
|
[41]
|
Wolf, M., O’Rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The Second Deficit: An Investigation of the Independence of Phonological and Naming-Speed Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 43-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1013816320290
|
[42]
|
Yap, L. R., & Van der Leij. (1994). Word Processing Dyslexics. An Automatic Decoding Deficit? Reading and Writing, 5, 261-279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01027391
|
[43]
|
Zeigler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading Acquisition, Developmental Dyslexia, and Skilled Reading across Languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
|