[1]
|
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279 362), New York, NY: Wiley and Sons.
|
[2]
|
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[3]
|
De Rijk, & Rudolf, P.G. (2007). Standard Basque: A progressive gram mar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[4]
|
Dryer, M. S. (2011). Order of subject, object, and verb. In M. Haspel math, M. S. Dryer, D. Gil, & B. Comrie (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.
http://wals.info/chapter/81
|
[5]
|
Erdozia, K., Laka, I., Mestres-Misse, A., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Syntactic complexity and ambiguity resolution in a free word order language: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidences from Basque. Brain and Language, 109, 1-17.
doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.12.003
|
[6]
|
Garnseya, S. M., Pearlmutterb, N. J., Myersa, Elizabeth, & Lotockyc, M. A. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Me mory and Language, 37, 58-93. doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2512
|
[7]
|
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of Language (pp. 40-70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[8]
|
Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[9]
|
Hawkins, J. A. (2003). Efficiency and complexity in grammars: Three general principles. In J. Moore, & M. Polinsky (Eds.), The Nature of Explanation in Linguistic Theory (pp. 121-152). Stanford: CSLI Pub lications, Stanford University.
|
[10]
|
Hualde, J. I., & Ortiz de Urbina, J. (2003). A Grammar of Basque. Ber lin: Mouton de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110895285
|
[11]
|
Kameyama, M. (1985). Zero anaphora: The case of Japanese. Doctoral Dissertation, Stanford, CA: Stanford University.
|
[12]
|
Kameyama, M. (1988). Japanese zero pronominal binding: Where syn tax and discourse meet. In W. J. Poser (Ed.), 2nd International Work shop on Japanese Syntax (pp. 47-73). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publica tions.
|
[13]
|
Laka, I. (1996). A brief grammar of Euskara, the Basque language.
www.ei.ehu.es/p289-content/eu/contenidos/informacion/grammar_euskara/en_doc/index.html
|
[14]
|
Lindsley, J. R. (1975). Producing simple utterances: How far ahead do we plan? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 1-19.
doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90002-X
|
[15]
|
MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-16.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226
|
[16]
|
Pickering, M. J. (1993). Direct association and sentence processing: A reply to Gorrell and to Gibson and Hickok. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 163-196. doi:10.1080/01690969308406953
|
[17]
|
Pickering, M. J., & Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without emp ty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 229-259.
doi:10.1080/01690969108406944
|
[18]
|
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical pref erence from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528-553.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.3.528
|
[19]
|
Prince, E. F. (1999). Subject pro-drop in Yiddish. In P. Bosch, & R. van der Sandt (Eds.), Focus: Linguistic, cognitive, and computatio nal perspectives (pp. 82-104). Cambridge, CA: Cambridge Universi ty Press.
|
[20]
|
Pritchett, B. L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing perfor mance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
|
[21]
|
Ros, I., Laka, I., Fukumura, K., & Santesteban, M. (2012). Long-be fore-short in head-final languages that agree. The Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP), Uni versidad di Trento.
|
[22]
|
Turan, ü. D. (1998). Ranking forward-looking centers in Turkish: Uni versal and language specific properties. In M. A. Walker, A. K. Joshi, & E. F. Prince (Eds.), Centering theory in discourse (pp. 139-161). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
|
[23]
|
Ueno, M., & Polinsky, M. (2009). Does headedness affect processing? A new look at the VO-OV contrast. Journal of Linguistics, 45, 675 710. doi:10.1017/S0022226709990065
|
[24]
|
Walker, M. A., Iida, M., & Cote, S. (1994). Japanese discourse and the process of centering. Computacional Linguistics, 20, 193-231.
|
[25]
|
Yamashita, H., & Chang, F. (2001). “Long before short” preference in the production of a head-final language. Cognition, 81, B45-B55.
doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00121-4
|
[26]
|
Yamashita, H., Chang, F., & Hirose, Y. (2005). Producers build struc tures only with overt arguments. The 18th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Tucson.
|
[27]
|
Yngve, V. H. (1960). A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 140, 444-466.
|