[1]
|
Acheson, A., Richards, J. B., & de Wit, H. (2007). Effects of sleep deprivation on impulsive behaviors in men and women. Physiology & Behavior, 91, 579-587. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.03.020
|
[2]
|
Arulampalam, W., Booth, A. L., & Bryan, M. L. (2007). Is there a glass ceiling over Europe? Exploring the gender pay gap across the wages distribution. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 60, 163-186.
|
[3]
|
Azmat, G., Guell, M., & Manning, A. (2006). Gender gaps in unemployment rates in OECD countries. Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 1-37. doi:10.1086/497817
|
[4]
|
Becker, G. S., & Mulligan, C. B. (1997). The endogenous determination of time preference. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 729- 758. doi:10.1162/003355397555334
|
[5]
|
Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice: Toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 482-488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011
|
[6]
|
Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Ad- diction, 96, 73-86. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x
|
[7]
|
Bonsang, E., Adam, S., & Perelman, S. (2012). Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? Journal of Health Economics, 31, 490-501.
doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.03.005
|
[8]
|
Chabris, C. F., Laibson, D., Morris, C. L., Schuldt, J. P., & Taubinsky, D. (2008). Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37, 237-269.
doi:10.1007/s11166-008-9053-x
|
[9]
|
Coe, N. B., & Zamarro, G., (2011). Retirement effects on health in Europe. Journal of Health Economics, 30, 77-86.
doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.11.002
|
[10]
|
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2005). Carpe diem: Adaptation and devaluing the future. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 55-61.
doi:10.1086/431025
|
[11]
|
Ernst, M. (2012). The usefulness of neuroeconomics for the study of depression across adolescence into adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 84-86. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.027
|
[12]
|
Fehr-Duda, H., De Gennaro, M., & Schubert, R. (2006). Gender, financial risk, and probability weights. Theory and Decision, 60, 283-313.
doi:10.1007/s11238-005-4590-0
|
[13]
|
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time dis- counting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351-401. doi:10.1257/002205102320161311
|
[14]
|
Fujisawa, T. X., Nishitani, S., Ishii, S., & Shinohara, K. (2011). Dif- ferential modulation of impulsive behavior by loneliness and testos- terone in adolescent females. Neuro Endocrinology Letters, 32, 836- 840.
|
[15]
|
Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33-36.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00610.x
|
[16]
|
Green, L., Myerson, J., & Ostaszewski, P. (1999). Discounting of delayed rewards across the life span: Age differences in individual discounting functions. Behavioural Processes, 46, 89-96.
doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00021-2
|
[17]
|
Hajali, V., Sheibani, V., Esmaeili-Mahani, S., & Shabani, M. (2012). Female rats are more susceptible to the deleterious effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on cognitive performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 228, 311-318. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.008
|
[18]
|
Ifcher, J., & Zarghamee, H. (2011). Happiness and time preference: The effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review, 101, 3109-3129.
doi:10.1257/aer.101.7.3109
|
[19]
|
Ikeda, S., Kang, M. I., & Ohtake, F. (2010). Hyperbolic discounting, the sign effect, and the body mass index. Journal of Health Econom- ics, 29, 268-284. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.01.002
|
[20]
|
Kimball, M. S., Sahm, C. R., & Shapiro, M. D. (2008). Imputing risk tolerance from survey responses. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 103, 1028-1038. doi:10.1198/016214508000000139
|
[21]
|
Kimmo, K., & Brent, S. (2010). Emotional reactions to losing explain gender differences in entering a risky lottery. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 159-163.
|
[22]
|
Kinari, Y., Ohtake, F., & Tsutsui, Y. (2009). Time discounting: De- clining impatience and interval effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 39, 87-112. doi:10.1007/s11166-009-9073-1
|
[23]
|
Kirby, K. N., & Marakovic, N. N. (1996). Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bul- letin & Review, 3, 100-104. doi:10.3758/BF03210748
|
[24]
|
Kirby, C., Winston, G., & Santiesteban, M. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213-222.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.003
|
[25]
|
L?ckenhoff, C. E. (2011). Age, time, and decision making: from processing speed to global time horizons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1235, 44-56. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06209.x
|
[26]
|
L?ckenhoff, C. E., O’Donoghue, T., & Dunning, D. (2011). Age differences in temporal discounting: The role of dispositional affect and anticipated emotions. Psychology and Aging, 26, 274-284.
doi:10.1037/a0023280
|
[27]
|
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272-292. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.0028
|
[28]
|
Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeconomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 647-672.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093710
|
[29]
|
McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503-507. doi:10.1126/science.1100907
|
[30]
|
Menz, M. M., Buchel, C., & Peters, J. (2012). Sleep deprivation is associated with attenuated parametric valuation and control signals in the midbrain during value-based decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6937-6946.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3553-11.2012
|
[31]
|
Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., & Kitamura, N. (2005). Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarette. Psychopharmacology, 182, 508-515.
doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8
|
[32]
|
Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66, 138-148.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026
|
[33]
|
Phillips, P. E., Kim, J. J., & Lee, D. (2012). Neuroeconomics. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6 . doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00015
|
[34]
|
Read, D., & Read, N. L. (2004). Time discounting over the lifespan. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94, 22-32.
doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.01.002
|
[35]
|
Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. M. (2004a). Impulsive choice and workplace safety: A new area of inquiry for research in occupational settings. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 239-246.
|
[36]
|
Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2004b). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: an experiential discounting task. Behavioural Processes, 67, 343-356.
|
[37]
|
Rogers, A. R. (1994). Evolution of time preference by natural selection. American Economic Review, 84, 460-481.
|
[38]
|
Sapienza, P., Zingales, L., & Maestripieri, D. (2009). Gender differences in financial risk aversion and career choices are affected by testosterone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15268-15273. doi:10.1073/pnas.0907352106
|
[39]
|
Scholten, M., & Read, D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117, 925-944.
doi:10.1037/a0019619
|
[40]
|
Schweighofer, N., Bertin, M., Shishida, K., Okamoto, Y., Tanaka, S. C., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K. (2008). Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4528-4532. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4982-07.2008
|
[41]
|
Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banish, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28-44.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x
|
[42]
|
Takahashi, T. (2004). Cortisol levels and time-discounting of monetary gain in humans. Neuroreport, 15, 2145-2147.
doi:10.1097/00001756-200409150-00029
|
[43]
|
Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Medical Hypotheses, 65, 691-693. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.040
|
[44]
|
Takahashi, T. (2009). Theoretical frameworks for neuroeconomics of intertemporal choice. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2, 75-90. doi:10.1037/a0015463
|
[45]
|
Takahashi, T., Ohmura, Y., Oono, H., & Radford, M. (2009). Alcohol use and discounting of delayed and probabilistic gain and loss. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 30, 749-752.
|
[46]
|
Takahashi, T., Oono, H., Inoue, T., Boku, S., Kako, Y., Kitaichi, Y., Kusumi, I., Masui, T., Nakagawa, S., Suzuki, K., Tanaka, T., Koyama, T., & Radford, M. H. (2008a). Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects—An analysis based on Tsallis’ statistics. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 29, 351-358.
|
[47]
|
Takahashi, T., Oono, H., & Radford, M. H. (2008b). Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Physica A, 387, 2066-2074. doi:10.1016/j.physa.2007.11.047
|
[48]
|
Takahashi, T., Sakaguchi, K., Oki, M., Homma, S., & Hasegawa, T. (2006). Testosterone levels and discounting delayed monetary gains and losses in male humans. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 27, 439- 444.
|
[49]
|
Takahashi, T., Shinada, M., Inukai, K., Tanida, S., Takahashi, C., Mi- fune, N., Takagishi, H., Horita, Y., Hashimoto, H., Yokota, K., Ka- meda, T., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Stress hormones predict hyperbolic time-discount rates six months later in adults. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 31, 616-621.
|
[50]
|
Venkatraman, V., Huettel, S. A., Chuah, L. Y., Payne, J. W., & Chee, M. W. (2011). Sleep deprivation biases the neural mechanisms underlying economic preferences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3712-3718. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4407-10.2011
|