Socio-Emotional Status, Education, and Time-Discounting in Japanese Non-Smoking Population: A Multi-Generational Study

Abstract

Recent studies in behavioral economics and neuroeconomics have revealed that emotion affects impulsivity in intertemporal choice. We examined the roles of socio-emotional status (i.e., perceived stress, depression, quality of sleep, loneliness) in temporal discounting behavior by Japanese non-smokers in a generation-specific manner (20 - 70 s) with a relatively large sample size (N = 3450). We observed that 1) both men and women are the most impulsive in their 60 s; 2) education has a negative impact on impulsivity in men aged 40 - 49 and women aged 50 - 59; 3) perceived stress has a negative impact on impulsivity in men aged 60 - 69; and 4) sleeplessness has negative and positive impacts on impulsivity in men aged 40 - 49 and women aged 30 - 39, respectively. Biological and social factors underlying observed findings are discussed.


Share and Cite:

Yamane, S. , Takahashi, T. , Kamesaka, A. , Tsutsui, Y. and Ohtake, F. (2013) Socio-Emotional Status, Education, and Time-Discounting in Japanese Non-Smoking Population: A Multi-Generational Study. Psychology, 4, 124-132. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.42018.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Acheson, A., Richards, J. B., & de Wit, H. (2007). Effects of sleep deprivation on impulsive behaviors in men and women. Physiology & Behavior, 91, 579-587. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.03.020
[2] Arulampalam, W., Booth, A. L., & Bryan, M. L. (2007). Is there a glass ceiling over Europe? Exploring the gender pay gap across the wages distribution. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 60, 163-186.
[3] Azmat, G., Guell, M., & Manning, A. (2006). Gender gaps in unemployment rates in OECD countries. Journal of Labor Economics, 24, 1-37. doi:10.1086/497817
[4] Becker, G. S., & Mulligan, C. B. (1997). The endogenous determination of time preference. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112, 729- 758. doi:10.1162/003355397555334
[5] Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice: Toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 482-488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011
[6] Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Ad- diction, 96, 73-86. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x
[7] Bonsang, E., Adam, S., & Perelman, S. (2012). Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? Journal of Health Economics, 31, 490-501. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.03.005
[8] Chabris, C. F., Laibson, D., Morris, C. L., Schuldt, J. P., & Taubinsky, D. (2008). Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37, 237-269. doi:10.1007/s11166-008-9053-x
[9] Coe, N. B., & Zamarro, G., (2011). Retirement effects on health in Europe. Journal of Health Economics, 30, 77-86. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.11.002
[10] Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2005). Carpe diem: Adaptation and devaluing the future. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 55-61. doi:10.1086/431025
[11] Ernst, M. (2012). The usefulness of neuroeconomics for the study of depression across adolescence into adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 84-86. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.027
[12] Fehr-Duda, H., De Gennaro, M., & Schubert, R. (2006). Gender, financial risk, and probability weights. Theory and Decision, 60, 283-313. doi:10.1007/s11238-005-4590-0
[13] Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time dis- counting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351-401. doi:10.1257/002205102320161311
[14] Fujisawa, T. X., Nishitani, S., Ishii, S., & Shinohara, K. (2011). Dif- ferential modulation of impulsive behavior by loneliness and testos- terone in adolescent females. Neuro Endocrinology Letters, 32, 836- 840.
[15] Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33-36. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00610.x
[16] Green, L., Myerson, J., & Ostaszewski, P. (1999). Discounting of delayed rewards across the life span: Age differences in individual discounting functions. Behavioural Processes, 46, 89-96. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00021-2
[17] Hajali, V., Sheibani, V., Esmaeili-Mahani, S., & Shabani, M. (2012). Female rats are more susceptible to the deleterious effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on cognitive performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 228, 311-318. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.008
[18] Ifcher, J., & Zarghamee, H. (2011). Happiness and time preference: The effect of positive affect in a random-assignment experiment. American Economic Review, 101, 3109-3129. doi:10.1257/aer.101.7.3109
[19] Ikeda, S., Kang, M. I., & Ohtake, F. (2010). Hyperbolic discounting, the sign effect, and the body mass index. Journal of Health Econom- ics, 29, 268-284. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.01.002
[20] Kimball, M. S., Sahm, C. R., & Shapiro, M. D. (2008). Imputing risk tolerance from survey responses. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 103, 1028-1038. doi:10.1198/016214508000000139
[21] Kimmo, K., & Brent, S. (2010). Emotional reactions to losing explain gender differences in entering a risky lottery. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 159-163.
[22] Kinari, Y., Ohtake, F., & Tsutsui, Y. (2009). Time discounting: De- clining impatience and interval effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 39, 87-112. doi:10.1007/s11166-009-9073-1
[23] Kirby, K. N., & Marakovic, N. N. (1996). Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bul- letin & Review, 3, 100-104. doi:10.3758/BF03210748
[24] Kirby, C., Winston, G., & Santiesteban, M. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213-222. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.003
[25] L?ckenhoff, C. E. (2011). Age, time, and decision making: from processing speed to global time horizons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1235, 44-56. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06209.x
[26] L?ckenhoff, C. E., O’Donoghue, T., & Dunning, D. (2011). Age differences in temporal discounting: The role of dispositional affect and anticipated emotions. Psychology and Aging, 26, 274-284. doi:10.1037/a0023280
[27] Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272-292. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.0028
[28] Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeconomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 647-672. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093710
[29] McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503-507. doi:10.1126/science.1100907
[30] Menz, M. M., Buchel, C., & Peters, J. (2012). Sleep deprivation is associated with attenuated parametric valuation and control signals in the midbrain during value-based decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6937-6946. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3553-11.2012
[31] Ohmura, Y., Takahashi, T., & Kitamura, N. (2005). Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarette. Psychopharmacology, 182, 508-515. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8
[32] Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66, 138-148. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026
[33] Phillips, P. E., Kim, J. J., & Lee, D. (2012). Neuroeconomics. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6 . doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00015
[34] Read, D., & Read, N. L. (2004). Time discounting over the lifespan. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94, 22-32. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.01.002
[35] Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. M. (2004a). Impulsive choice and workplace safety: A new area of inquiry for research in occupational settings. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 239-246.
[36] Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2004b). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: an experiential discounting task. Behavioural Processes, 67, 343-356.
[37] Rogers, A. R. (1994). Evolution of time preference by natural selection. American Economic Review, 84, 460-481.
[38] Sapienza, P., Zingales, L., & Maestripieri, D. (2009). Gender differences in financial risk aversion and career choices are affected by testosterone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15268-15273. doi:10.1073/pnas.0907352106
[39] Scholten, M., & Read, D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117, 925-944. doi:10.1037/a0019619
[40] Schweighofer, N., Bertin, M., Shishida, K., Okamoto, Y., Tanaka, S. C., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K. (2008). Low-serotonin levels increase delayed reward discounting in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4528-4532. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4982-07.2008
[41] Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banish, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28-44. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x
[42] Takahashi, T. (2004). Cortisol levels and time-discounting of monetary gain in humans. Neuroreport, 15, 2145-2147. doi:10.1097/00001756-200409150-00029
[43] Takahashi, T. (2005). Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Medical Hypotheses, 65, 691-693. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.040
[44] Takahashi, T. (2009). Theoretical frameworks for neuroeconomics of intertemporal choice. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2, 75-90. doi:10.1037/a0015463
[45] Takahashi, T., Ohmura, Y., Oono, H., & Radford, M. (2009). Alcohol use and discounting of delayed and probabilistic gain and loss. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 30, 749-752.
[46] Takahashi, T., Oono, H., Inoue, T., Boku, S., Kako, Y., Kitaichi, Y., Kusumi, I., Masui, T., Nakagawa, S., Suzuki, K., Tanaka, T., Koyama, T., & Radford, M. H. (2008a). Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects—An analysis based on Tsallis’ statistics. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 29, 351-358.
[47] Takahashi, T., Oono, H., & Radford, M. H. (2008b). Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Physica A, 387, 2066-2074. doi:10.1016/j.physa.2007.11.047
[48] Takahashi, T., Sakaguchi, K., Oki, M., Homma, S., & Hasegawa, T. (2006). Testosterone levels and discounting delayed monetary gains and losses in male humans. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 27, 439- 444.
[49] Takahashi, T., Shinada, M., Inukai, K., Tanida, S., Takahashi, C., Mi- fune, N., Takagishi, H., Horita, Y., Hashimoto, H., Yokota, K., Ka- meda, T., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Stress hormones predict hyperbolic time-discount rates six months later in adults. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 31, 616-621.
[50] Venkatraman, V., Huettel, S. A., Chuah, L. Y., Payne, J. W., & Chee, M. W. (2011). Sleep deprivation biases the neural mechanisms underlying economic preferences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3712-3718. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4407-10.2011

Copyright © 2025 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.