[1]
|
Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) during Childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71-82.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
|
[2]
|
Archibald, S. J., & Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and Description of New Tests of Executive Functioning in Children. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 115-129.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/chin.5.2.115.3167
|
[3]
|
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral Inhibition, Sustained Attention, and Executive Functions: Constructing a Unifying Theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65
|
[4]
|
Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive Functions in Children Aged 6 to 13: A Dimensional and Developmental Study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 571-593.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3
|
[5]
|
Catale, C., & Meulemans, T. (2009). The Real Animal Size Test (RAST): A New Measure of Inhibitory Control for Young Children. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 83-91.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.25.2.83
|
[6]
|
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the Control of Automatic Processes: A Parallel Distributed Processing Account of the Stroop Effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332-361.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332
|
[7]
|
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
|
[8]
|
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The Relations among Inhibition and Interference Control Functions: A Latent-Vari- able Analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101-135.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
|
[9]
|
Ikeda, Y., Hirata, S., Maeda, W., Okuzumi, H., Kokubun, M., Ambe, H., Fujieda, Y., Suijo, K., Hayashi, J., & Iteya, M. (2013) Relationship between Stroop/Reverse-Stroop Interference and Postural Sway When Standing in Elderly People. Comprehensive Psychology, 2, 9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/22.25.CP.2.9
|
[10]
|
Ikeda, Y., Hirata, S., Okuzumi, H., & Kokubun, M. (2010). Features of Stroop and Reverse-Stroop Interference: Analysis by Response Modality and Evaluation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110, 654-660.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.110.2.654-660
|
[11]
|
Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H., & Kokubun, M. (2013a). Age-Related Trends of Stroop-Like Interference in Animal Size Tests in 5- to 12-Year-Old Children and Young Adults. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 276-291.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.658364
|
[12]
|
Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H., & Kokubun, M. (2013b). Stroop/Reverse-Stroop Interference in Typical Development and Its Relation to Symptoms of ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2391-2398.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.019
|
[13]
|
Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H., Kokubun, M., & Haishi, K. (2011). Age-Related Trends of Interference Control in School-Age Children and Young Adults in the Stroop Color-Word Test. Psychological Reports, 108, 577-584.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/04.10.22.PR0.108.2.577-584
|
[14]
|
Loher, S., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Executive Functions and Their Differential Contribution to Sustained Attention in 5- to 8-Year-Old Children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 3, 51-63.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/jedp.v3n1p51
|
[15]
|
Ludwig, C., Borella, E., Tettamanti, M., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2010). Adult Age Differences in the Color Stroop Test: A Comparison between an Item-by-Item and a Blocked Version. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 51, 135-142.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.040
|
[16]
|
Macdonald, J. A., Beauchamp, M. H., Crigan, J. A., & Anderson, P. J. (2014). Age-Related Differences in Inhibitory Control in the Early School Years. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 509-526.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2013.822060
|
[17]
|
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a Century of Research on the Stroop Effect: An Integrative Review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
|
[18]
|
McDowd, J. M., Oseas-Kreger, D. M., & Filion, D. L. (1995). Inhibitory Processes in Cognition and Aging. In F. N. Dempster, & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and Inhibition in Cognition (pp. 363-400). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012208930-5/50012-X
|
[19]
|
Michel, E., & Roebers, C. M. (2008). Children in Regular and Special Needs Classes: Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Aspects. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 249-259.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.67.4.249
|
[20]
|
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8-14.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
|
[21]
|
Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Röthlisberger, M., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Personality Factors in Elementary School Children: Contributions to Academic Performance over and above Executive Functions? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 118-125.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.006
|
[22]
|
Nichelli, F., Scala, G., Vago, C., Riva, D., & Bulgheroni, S. (2005). Age-Related Trends in Stroop and Conflicting Motor Response Task Findings. Child Neuropsychology, 11, 431-443.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297040590951569
|
[23]
|
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On Inhibition/Disinhibition in Developmental Psychopathology: Views from Cognitive and Personality Psychology and a Working Inhibition Taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220-246.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220
|
[24]
|
Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief Report: Specific Executive Function Profiles in Three Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171-177.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023052913110
|
[25]
|
Prevor, M. B., & Diamond, A. (2005). Color-Object Interference in Young Children: A Stroop Effect in Children 3½ - 6½ Years Old. Cognitive Development, 20, 256-278.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.04.001
|
[26]
|
Pritchatt, D. (1968). An Investigation into Some of the Underlying Associative Verbal Processes of the Stroop Colour Effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 351-359.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640746808400174
|
[27]
|
Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., & Jäger, K. (2014). The Relation between Cognitive and Motor Performance and Their Relevance for Children’s Transition to School: A Latent Variable Approach. Human Movement Science, 33, 284-297.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.011
|
[28]
|
Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Executive Functions in 5- to 8-Year Olds: Developmental Changes and Relationship to Academic Achievement. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 3, 153-167.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/jedp.v3n2p153
|
[29]
|
Song, Y., & Hakoda, Y. (2011). An Asymmetric Stroop/Reverse-Stroop Interference Phenomenon in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 499-505.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054710367607
|
[30]
|
Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D. (2003). A New Stroop-Like Measure of Inhibitory Function Development: Typical Developmental Trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 561-575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00145
|
[31]
|
Yasumura, A., Kokubo, N., Yamamoto, H., Yasumura, Y., Nakagawa, E., Kaga, M., Hiraki, K., & Inagaki, M. (2014). Neurobehavioral and Hemodynamic Evaluation of Stroop and Reverse Stroop Interference in Children with Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder. Brain and Development, 36, 97-106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2013.01.005
|