[1]
|
Piaget, J. (1932) The Moral Judgment of the Child. The Free Press, New York.
|
[2]
|
Haidt, J. (2001) The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814
|
[3]
|
Greene, J.D. (2007) Why Are VMPFC Patients More Utilitarian? A Dual-Process Theory of Moral Judgment Explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 322-323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004
|
[4]
|
Suter, R.S. and Hertwig, R. (2011) Time and Moral Judgment. Cognition, 119, 454-458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.018
|
[5]
|
Starcke, K., Polzer, C., Wolf, O.T. and Brand, M. (2011) Does Stress Alter Everyday Moral Decision-Making? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 210-219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.010
|
[6]
|
Plettia, C., Sarloa, M., Palombaa, D., Rumiatib, R. and Lottob, L. (2015) Evaluation of the Legal Consequences of Action Affects Neural Activity and Emotional Experience during the Resolution of Moral Dilemmas. Brain and Cognition, 94, 24-31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.01.004
|
[7]
|
Grafman, J. and Wood, J.N. (2003) Human Prefrontal Cortex: Processing and Representational Perspectives. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 139-147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1033
|
[8]
|
Grafman, J., Wood, J.N., Romero, S.G. and Knutson, K.M. (2005) Representation of Attitudinal Knowledge: Role of Prefrontal Cortex, Amygdala and Parahippocampalgyrus. Neuropsychologia, 43, 249-259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.011
|
[9]
|
Greene, J. (2003) From Neural “Is” to Moral “Ought”: What Are the Moral Implications of Neuroscientific Moral Psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 846-849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1224
|
[10]
|
Greene, J. (2005) Emotion and Cognition in Moral Judgment: Evidence from Neuroimaging. Neurobiology of Human Values, 57-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/3-540-29803-7_6
|
[11]
|
Greene, J. (2009) La Peyrouse in the Island of Tahiti, or the Danger of Presumption: Political and Moral Drama in Four Acts. Journal for Eighteenth-Century Studies, 32, 627-628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1754-0208.2008.00132.x
|
[12]
|
Greene, J. and Haidt, J. (2002) How (and Where) Does Moral Judgment Work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517-523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-9
|
[13]
|
Greene, J.D., Cushman, F.A., Stewart, L.E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L.E. and Cohen, J.D. (2009) Pushing Moral Buttons: The Interaction between Personal Force and Intention in Moral Judgment. Cognition, 111, 364-371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.001
|
[14]
|
Hongwei, Z. and Ye, L. (2014) Moral Behavior under Two Kinds of Moral Self-Regulation Mechanisms. Advances in Psychological Science, 22, 1178-1187.
|
[15]
|
Yodera, J.K. and Decety, J. (2014) Spatiotemporal Neural Dynamics of Moral Judgment: A High-Density ERP Study. Neuropsychologia, 60, 39-45.
|
[16]
|
Sarlo, M., Lotto, L., Manfrinati, A., Rumiati, R., Gallicchio, G. and Palomba, D. (2011) Temporal Dynamics of Cognitive-Emotional Interplay in Moral Decision-Making. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1018-1029. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00146
|
[17]
|
Vicentea, R., Susemihlb, A., Jericóc, J.P. and Caticha, N. (2015) Moral Foundations in an Interacting Neural Networks Society: A Statistical Mechanics Analysis. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications, 400, 124-138.
|