Memory Suggestibility under Social Pressure and Inhibitory Control in Preschoolers

Abstract

The current study examined the interaction of inhibitory control and social pressure on young children’s memory suggestibility. Children were administered the Video Memory Suggestibility Scale for Children (VMSSC) and batteries of inhibitory control tasks, and were exposed to different levels of situational social pressures during interviews for memory suggestibility. The findings indicated that inhibitory control played a significantly moderating role in the relationship between social pressure and memory suggestibility. The greater the inhibitory control, the lower the effects of social pressure on the memory suggestibility of children. For the children with lower levels of inhibitory control, the social pressure positively predicted memory suggestibility; but for the children with higher levels of inhibitory control, the social pressure didn’t significantly predict memory suggestibility. So, to a certain extent, memory suggestibility may be a dynamic integration of stable trait and fluctuating state elements. The implications for the essence of memory suggestibility were discussed.

Share and Cite:

Cao, X. (2015) Memory Suggestibility under Social Pressure and Inhibitory Control in Preschoolers. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 5, 121-129. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2015.54012.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Bruck, M., Ceci, S.J. and Melnyk, L. (1997) External and Internal Sources of Variation in the Creation of False Reports in Children. Learning and Individual Differences, 9, 289-316.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(97)90011-X
[2] Bruck, M. and Melnyk, L. (2004) Individual Differences in Children’s Suggestibility: A Review and Synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 947-996.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1070
[3] Finnila, K., Mahlberg, N., Santtila, P., Sandnabba, K. and Niemi, P. (2003) Validity of a Test of Children’s Suggestibility for Predicting Responses to Two Interview Situations Differing in Degree of Suggestiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 32-49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00025-0
[4] Rothbart, M.K., Ellis, L.K., Rueda, M.R. and Posner, M.I. (2003) Developing Mechanisms of Temperamental Effortful Control. Journal of Personality, 71, 1113-1143.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.7106009
[5] Alexander, K.W., Goodman, G.S., Schaaf, J.M., Edelstein, R.S., Quas, J.A. and Shaver, P.R. (2002) The Role of Attachment and Cognitive Inhibition in Children’s Memory and Suggestibility for a Stressful Event. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 262-290.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00149-2
[6] Melinder, A., Endestad, T. and Magnussen, S. (2006) Relations between Episodic Memory, Suggestibility, Theory of mind, and Cognitive Inhibition in the Preschool Child. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 485-495.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00542.x
[7] Roberts, K.P. and Powell, M.B. (2005) The Relation between Inhibitory Control and Children’s Eyewitness Memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1003-1018.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1141
[8] Wei, Y.G., Wu, R.M. and Li, H. (2005) Role of Inhibitory Control in Infants’ Executive Function and Theory of Mind. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 37, 598-605.
[9] Perner, J. and Lang, B. (1999) Development of Theory of Mind and Executive Control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 337-344.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01362-5
[10] Scullin, M.H., Kanaya, T. and Ceci, S.J. (2002) Measurement of Individual Differences in Children’s Suggestibility across Situations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 233-246.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//1076-898X.8.4.233
[11] Zelazo, P.D., Müller, U., Frye, D. and Marcovitch, S. (2003) The Development of Executive Function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 1-27.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x
[12] Scullin, M.H. and Bonner, K. (2006) Theory of Mind, Inhibitory Control, and Preschool-Age Children’s Suggestibility in Different Interviewing Contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 120-138.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.09.005
[13] Schaaf, J.M., Alexander, K.W. and Goodman, G.S. (2008) Children’s False Memory and True Disclosure in the Face of Repeated Questions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100, 157-185.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.09.002
[14] Roebers, C.M. and Schneider, W. (2000) The Impact of Misleading Questions on Eyewitness Memory in Children and Adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 509-526.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1099-0720(200011/12)14:6<509::AID-ACP668>3.0.CO;2-W
[15] Quas, J.A. and Schaaf, J.M. (2002) Children’s Memories of Experienced and Nonexperienced Events Following Repeated Interviews. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 304-338.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00150-9
[16] Garven, S., Wood, J.M. and Malpass, R.S. (2000) Allegations of Wrongdoing: The Effects of Reinforcement on Children’s Mundane and Fantastic Claims. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 38-49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.85.1.38
[17] Levine, L.J., Burgess, S.L. and Laney, C. (2008) Effects of Discrete Emotions on Young Children’s Suggestibility. Developmental Psychology, 44, 681-694.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.681
[18] Price, H.L. and Connolly, D.A. (2007) Anxious and Nonanxious Children’s Recall of a Repeated or Unique Event. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 94-112.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.05.002
[19] Karpinski, A.C. and Scullin, M.H. (2009) Suggestibility under Pressure: Theory of Mind, Executive Function, and Suggestibility in Preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 749-763.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2009.05.004
[20] Gerstadt, C.L., Hong, Y.J. and Diamond, A. (1994) The Relationship between Cognition and Action: Performance of Children 3.5-7 Years Old on a Stroop-Like Day-Night Test. Cognition, 53, 129-153.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
[21] Carlson, S.M. and Moses, L.J. (2001) Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control and Children’s Theory of Mind. Child Development, 72, 1032-1053.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333
[22] Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T.Y., Koenig, A.L. and Vandegeest, K.A. (1996) Inhibitory Control in Young Children and Its Role in Emerging Internalization. Child Development, 67, 490-507.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131828
[23] Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J. and Claxton, L.J. (2004) Individual Differences in Executive Functioning and Theory of Mind: An Investigation of Inhibitory Control and Planning Ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299-319.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002
[24] Gudjonsson, G.H. (1992) Interrogative Suggestibility: Factor Analysis of the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (GSS 2). Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 479-481.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(92)90077-3
[25] Ceci, S.J. and Bruck, M. (1993) Suggestibility of the Child Witness: A Historical Review and Synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403-439.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.403
[26] Roebers, C.M. and Schneider, W. (2002) Stability and Consistency of Children’s Event Recall. Cognitive Development, 17, 1085-1103.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00075-8
[27] Lehman, E.B., McKinley, M.J., Thompson, D.W., Leonard, A.M., Liebmanm, J.I. and Rothrock, D.D. (2010) Long-Term Stability of Young Children’s Eyewitness Accuracy, Suggestibility, and Resistance to Misinformation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 145-154.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2009.11.007

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.