[1]
|
Ashford, K., & Jackson, R. (2010). Priming as a Means of Preventing Skill Failure under Pressure. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32, 518-536.
|
[2]
|
Balentine, C. B., & Brownlow, S. (2006). Does Making Salient Task Relevance to Group Affiliation Decrease the Performance of Male Athletes on Spatial Tasks? Psi Chi Journal, 11, 37-44.
|
[3]
|
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230-244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
|
[4]
|
Bodner, G. M., & Guay, R. B. (1997). The Purdue Visualization of Rotations Test. The Chemical Educator, 2, 1-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00897970138a
|
[5]
|
Brownlow, S., Janas, A. J., Blake, K. A., Rebadow, K. T., & Mellon, L. M. (2011). Getting by with a Little Help from My Friends: Mental Rotation Ability after Tacit Peer Encouragement. Psychology, 2, 383-370. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2011.24057
|
[6]
|
Brownlow, S., Valentine, S. E., & Owusu, A. (2008). Women Athletes’ Mental Rotation under Stereotypic Threat. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107, 307-316. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.107.1.307-316
|
[7]
|
Carr, P. B., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Stereotype Threat and Inflexible Perseverance in Problem Solving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 853-859. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.003
|
[8]
|
Chambon, M. (2009). Embodied Perception with Other’s Bodies in Mind: Stereotype Priming Influence on the Perception of Spatial Environments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 283-287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.023
|
[9]
|
Cherney, I. D. (2008). Mom, Let Me Play More Computer Games: They Improve My Mental Rotation Skills. Sex Roles, 59, 776-786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9498-z
|
[10]
|
Cherney, I. D., & Collaer, M. L. (2005). Sex Differences in Line Judgment: Relationship to Mathematics Preparation and Strategy Use. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, 615-627. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PMS.100.3.615-627
|
[11]
|
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-National Patterns of Gender Differences in Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103-127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018053
|
[12]
|
Goldstein, D., Haldane, D., & Mitchell, C. (1990). Sex Differences in Visual-Spatial Ability: The Role of Performance Factors. Memory and Cognition, 18, 546-550. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03198487
|
[13]
|
Gonzalez, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002). The Effects of Stereotype Threat and Double Minority Status on the Test Performance of Latino Women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 656-670.
|
[14]
|
Gresky, D. M., Ten Eyck, L. L., Lord, C. G., & McIntyre, R. B. (2005). Effects of Salient Multiple Identities on Women’s Performance under Mathematics Stereotype Threat. Sex Roles, 53, 703-715. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-7735-2
|
[15]
|
Kanoy, A., Brownlow, S., & Sowers, T. F. (2012). Can Rewards Obviate Stereotype Threat Effects on Mental Rotation Tasks? Psychology, 3, 542-547. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2012.37080
|
[16]
|
Maeda, Y., & Yoon, Y. S. (2013). A Meta-Analysis on Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability Measured by the Purdue Spatial Visualization Tests: Visualization of Rotations (PSVT:R). Educational Psychology Review, 25, 69-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9215-x
|
[17]
|
McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating Women’s Mathematics Stereotype Threat through Salience of Group Achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 83-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9
|
[18]
|
O’Brien, L. T., & Crandall, C. S. (2003). Stereotype Threat and Arousal: Effects on Women’s Math Performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 782-789. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029006010
|
[19]
|
Ortner, T. M., & Sieverding, M. (2008). Where Are the Gender Differences? Male Priming Boosts Spatial Skills in Women. Sex Roles, 59, 274-281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9448-9
|
[20]
|
Ozel, S., Larue, L., & Molinaro, C. (2004). Relationship between Sport and Spatial Imagery: Comparison of Three Group of Participants. Journal of Psychology, 138, 49-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.138.1.49-64
|
[21]
|
Scali, R. M., Brownlow, S., & Hicks, J. (2000). Gender Differences in Spatial Task Performance as a Function of Speed or Accuracy Orientation. Sex Roles, 43, 359-376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1026699310308
|
[22]
|
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender Identification Moderates Stereotype Threat Effects on Women’s Math Performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194-201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1500
|
[23]
|
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An Integrated Process Model of Stereotype Effects on Performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336-356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336
|
[24]
|
Shapiro, J. R., & Williams, A. M. (2012). The Role of Stereotype Threats in Undermining Girls’ and Women’s Performance and Interest in STEM Fields. Sex Roles, 66, 175-183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0051-0
|
[25]
|
Sharps, M. J., Price, J. R., & Williams, J. K. (1994). Spatial Cognition and Gender: Instructional and Stimulus Influences on Mental Image Rotation Performance. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 413-425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb00464.x
|
[26]
|
Steele, C. M. (1997). A Threat in the Air: How Stereotypes Shape Intellectual Identity and Performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613-629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
|
[27]
|
Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype Threat Effects of Black and White Athletic Performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1213-1227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213
|
[28]
|
Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The Malleability of Spatial Skills: A Meta-Analysis of Training Studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352-402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028446
|
[29]
|
Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978.) Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599-604. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599
|
[30]
|
Voyer, D., & Isaacs, M. (1993). Sex Differences in Mental Rotation: Role of Practice and Experience. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior, and Cognitive Science.
|
[31]
|
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: A Meta-Analysis and Consideration of Critical Variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250-270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250
|