Expressing Food through Art: Evidence for a Nutrient-Specific Effect on Mood

Abstract

Background: Brain imaging studies show evidence of selective brain reward responses to high calorie foods. Behavioral studies extend this research by showing that such foods can enhance emotions, even for sweet-tasting low calorie foods (i.e., fruits). In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that participants will show more positive emotional change when drawing pictures of foods that are high fat or taste sweet compared to bitter-tasting foods—as a possible behavioral intervention for enhancing mood. Method: Participants were randomly assigned to one of four art groups: high fat-high sugar (HFHS; stimulus food: cupcakes), high fat-low sugar (HFLS; stimulus food: pizza), low fat-high sugar (LFHS; stimulus food: strawberries), or low fat-low sugar (LFLS; stimulus food: peppers). Participants used three colors (red, green, black) in their art, were required to use all three colors, and told that the colors they use must reflect actual colors that are natural for the food depicted. Participants drew images of a stimulus food and prepost measures of mood and arousal were recorded. Results: Consistent with the hypothesis, the results show that drawing pictures of high fat foods (cupcakes, pizzas) and a food that tastes sweet (strawberries) results in greater increases in mood compared to drawing a bitter-tasting food (peppers). Changes in mood were independent of BMI, daily sugar intake, daily fat intake, arousal, and hunger. Conclusion: These results extend a growing body of biobehavioral research on the positive impact of food images on mood by showing that this impact can be applied to enhance mood when expressing food images through art.

Share and Cite:

G. Privitera, B. Moshaty, F. Marzullo and M. Misenheimer, "Expressing Food through Art: Evidence for a Nutrient-Specific Effect on Mood," Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, Vol. 3 No. 2, 2013, pp. 163-167. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2013.32016.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] E. Diener, “Subjective Well-Being: The Science of Happiness and a Proposal for a National Index,” The American Psychologist, Vol. 55, No. 1, 2000, pp. 34-43. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34
[2] J. J. Gross, “The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review,” Review of General Psychology, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1998, pp. 271-299. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
[3] R. J. Larsen, “Toward a Science of Mood Regulation,” Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 11, No. 3, 2000, pp. 129-141.
[4] M. E. P. Seligman and M. Csikszentmihalyi, “Positive Psychology: An Introduction,” The American Psychologist, Vol. 55, No. 1, 2000, pp. 5-14. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5
[5] C. S. Carver and M. F. Scheier, “Origins and Function of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control Process View,” Psychological Review, Vol. 97, No. 1, 1990, pp. 19-35. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19
[6] R. J. Larsen and E. Diener, “Affect Intensity as an Individual Difference Characteristic: A Review,” Journal of Research in Personality, Vol. 21, No. 1, 1987, pp. 1-39. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(87)90023-7
[7] J. M. Zelinsky and R. J. Larsen, “Susceptibility to Affect. A Comparison of Three Personality Taxonomies,” Journal of Personality, Vol. 67, No. 5, 1999, pp. 761-791. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00072
[8] D. M. Buss, “The Evolution of Happiness,” The American Psychologist, Vol. 55, No. 1, 2000, pp. 15-23. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.15
[9] A. Dalebroux, T. R. Goldstein and E. Winner, “ShortTerm Mood Repair through Art-Making: Positive Emotion Is More Effective than Venting,” Motivation & Emotion, Vol. 32, No. 4, 2008, pp. 288-295. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9105-1
[10] C. Dere-Meyer, B. Bender, E. Metzl and K. Diaz, “Psychotropic Medication and Art Therapy: Overview of Literature and Clinical Considerations,” The Arts in Psychotherapy, Vol. 38, No. 1, 2011, pp. 29-35. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2010.10.003
[11] A. Byers, “Visual Aesthetics in Dementia,” International Journal of Art Therapy, Vol. 16, No. 2, 2011, pp. 81-89. doi:10.1080/17454832.2011.602980
[12] S. Frank, N. Laharnar, S. Kullmann, R. Veit, C. Canova, Y. L. Hegner, et al., “Processing of Food Pictures: Influence of Hunger, Gender and Calorie Content,” Brain Research, Vol. 1350, 2010, pp. 159-166. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.030
[13] G. J. Privitera and H. E. Creary, “Proximity and Visibility of Fruits and Vegetables Influences Intake in a Kitchen Setting among College Students,” Environment and Behavior, Vol. 45, No. 5, 2013. doi:10.1177/0013916512442892
[14] G. J. Privitera, D. E. Antonelli and H. E. Creary, “The Effect of Food Images on Mood and Arousal Depends on Dietary Histories and the Fat and Sugar Content of Foods Depicted,” Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, Vol. 3, No. 1, 2013, pp. 1-6. doi:10.4236/jbbs.2013.31001
[15] A. W. Logue, “The Psychology of Eating and Drinking, 3rd Edition, Brunner-Routledge, New York, 2004.
[16] M. Macht and G. Simons, “Emotions and Eating in Everyday Life,” Appetite, Vol. 35, No. 1, 2000, pp. 65-71. doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0325
[17] B. Wansink, M. M. Cheney and N. Chan, “Exploring Comfort Food Preferences across Age and Gender,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 79, No. 4-5, 2003, pp. 739-747. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00203-8
[18] L. Dubé, J. L. LeBel and J. Lu, “Affect Asymmetry and Comfort Food Consumption,” Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 86, No. 4, 2005, pp. 559-567. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.023
[19] M. S. Faith, D. B. Allison and A. Geliebter, “Emotional Eating and Obesity: Theoretical Considerations and Practical Recommendations,” In: S. Dalton, Ed., Overweight and Weight Management: The Health Professional’s Guide to Understanding and Practice, Aspen Publishers, Gaithersburg, 1997, pp. 439-465.
[20] S. C. Sanderson, “Obesity Risk,” In: K. P. Tercyak, Ed., Handbook of Genomics and the Family: Psychosocial Context for Children and Adolescents, Springer Science + Business Media, New York, 2010, pp. 329-343.
[21] W. D. Killgore, A. D. Young, L. A. Femia, P. Bogorodzki, J. Rogowska and D. A. Yurgelun-Todd, “Cortical and Limbic Activation during Viewing of Highversus LowCalorie Foods,” Neuroimage, Vol. 19, No. 4, 2003, pp. 1381-1394. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00191-5
[22] W. K. Simmons, A. Martin and L. W. Barsalou, “Pictures of Appetizing Foods Activate Gustatory Cortices for Taste and Reward,” Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 15, No. 10, 2005, pp. 1602-1608. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi038
[23] G. J. Wang, N. D. Volkow, F. Telang, M. Jayne, J. Ma, M. Rao, et al., “Exposure to Appetitive Food Stimuli Markedly Activates the Human Brain,” Neuroimage, Vol. 21, No. 4, 2004, pp. 1790-1797. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.026
[24] B. Fletcher, K. J. Pine, Z. Woodridge and A. Nash, “How Visual Images of Chocolate Affect the Craving and Guilt of Female Dieters,” Appetite, Vol. 48, No. 2, 2007, pp. 211-217. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.09.002
[25] D. D. Wagner, R. G. Boswell, W. M. Kelley and T. F. Heatherton, “Inducing Negative Affect Increases the Reward Value of Appetizing Foods in Dieters,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 24, No. 7, 2012, pp. 16251633. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00238
[26] B. Wansink, J. E. Painter and Y.-K. Lee, “The Office Candy Dish: Proximity’s Influence on Estimated and Actual Consumption,” International Journal of Obesity, Vol. 30, No. 5, 2006, pp. 871-875. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803217
[27] H. Yoshimura, M. Honjo, T. Sugai, M. Kawabe, K. Kaneyama, N. Segami and N. Kato, “Influences of Audio-Visual Environments on Feelings of Deliciousness during Having Sweet Foods: An Electroence-phalogram Frequency Analysis Study,” Nutritional Neuroscience, Vol. 14, No. 5, 2011, pp. 210-215.
[28] G. J. Wang, N. D. Volkow, F. Telang, M. Jayne, Y. Ma, K. Pradhan, et al., “Evidence of Gender Differences in the Ability to Inhibit Brain Activation Elicited by Food Stimulation,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 106, No. 4, 2009, pp. 1249-1254.
[29] J. A. Russell, A. Weiss and G. A. Mendelsohn, “Affect Grid: A Single-Item Scale of Pleasure and Arousal,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 57, No. 3, 1989, pp. 493-502. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.493
[30] G. J. Privitera and M. Wallace, “An Assessment of Liking for Sugars Using the Estimated Daily Intake Scale,” Appetite, Vol. 56, No. 1, 2011, pp. 713-718. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.008
[31] G. J. Privitera and C. S. Freeman, “Validity and Reliability of an Estimated Daily Intake Scale for Fat,” Global Journal of Health Science, Vol. 4, No. 2, 2012, pp. 36-41. doi:10.5539/gjhs.v4n2p36
[32] K. Seong-In, “A Computer System for the Analysis of Color-Related Elements in Art Therapy Assessment: Computer-Color-Related Elements Art Therapy Evaluation System (C_CREATES),” The Arts & Psychotherapy, Vol. 37, 2010, pp. 378-386. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2010.09.002
[33] B. J. Tepper, T. Z. A. Williams, J. R. Burgess, C. J. Antalis and R. D. Mattes, “Genetic Variation in Bitter Taste and Plasma Markers of Anti-Oxidant Status in College Women,” International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition, Vol. 60, No. S2, 2009, pp. 35-45. doi:10.1080/09637480802304499

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.