Optimal Performance in Means-Ends Activities: On the Emergence of Self-Other/World Differentiation in Infancy

Abstract

We know little about the role of background positive affectivity and flow experiences in early development concerning means-ends activities and their possible contribution regarding symbolic functioning and the consolidation of the self as agent. The theoretical argument presented here is that a deeper appreciation of the benefits and challenges of optimal experience may help built a more unified account of optimal functioning in development. That is, the paradox of control in flow, and its relational/mirroring structure, wherein the demands of the structure (means) are entities in themselves (ends), mark possibilities for the temporal organization of experience when fine motor tuning and gross motor intention overlap. When means-focused action/temporal regulation (I) and ends-focused action (Me) become synchronized in an integrated embodied system, the system shifts experience to the experiential gear. Means-ends dynamics, then, via mirroring processes organized in turn-taking flow structures between formative action (means) and production, push the child’s primary reactive space to be converted into the child’s interactive experiential space and in turn mapped on and gradually transformed to the “secondary,” communicative/expressive space, which also controls its manifestation, initiating in line conscious control and self-referential intentionality.

Share and Cite:

Stamatopoulou, D. (2012). Optimal Performance in Means-Ends Activities: On the Emergence of Self-Other/World Differentiation in Infancy. Psychology, 3, 1059-1066. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.312157.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Adolph, K. E., & Berger, S. E. (2006). Motor development. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegel (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2: Cognition, perception and language (6th ed., pp. 161-213). New York: Wiley.
[2] Apter, M. (1984). Reversal theory, cognitive synergy and the arts. In W. R. Crozier, & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in the perception of art (pp. 411-426). Amsterdam: North Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62361-4
[3] Baldwin, J. M. (1894). Imitation: A chapter in the natural history of consciousness. Mind, 3, 26-55. doi:10.1093/mind/III.9.26
[4] Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122
[5] Bard, K. (2008). Understanding reflections of self and other objects. In C. Lange-Kuttner, & A. Vinter (Eds.), Drawing and the non-verbal mind (pp.23-41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489730.002
[6] Brandstadter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 807-863). New York: Wiley.
[7] Camaioni, L., Aureli, T., Bellagamba, F., & Fogel, A. (2003). A longitudinal examination of the transition to symbolic communication in the second year of life. Infant and Child Development, 12, 1-26. doi:10.1002/icd.333
[8] Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mind-brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[9] Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.
[10] Csikszentmihalyi M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I., (1988). Optimal experience. Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511621956
[11] Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. New York: Harper & Row.
[12] Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic Books.
[13] Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998). The development of the person: An experiential perspective on the ontogenesis of psychological complexity. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 635-684). New York: Wiley.
[14] Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Massimini, F. (1985). On the psychological selection of bio-cultural information. New Ideas in Psychology, 3, 115-138. doi:10.1016/0732-118X(85)90002-9
[15] Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Robinson, R. E. (1990). The art of seeing. Malibu, CA: The Paul Getty Trust Office of Publications.
[16] Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2003). Drug addiction: The paradox of mimetic optimal experience. In J. Henry (Ed.), European positive psychology proceedings (pp. 31-38). Leicester: British Psychological Society.
[17] Delle Fave, A. (2007). Individual development and community empowerment: Suggestions from studies on optimal experience. In J. Haworth & G. Hart (Eds.), Well-being: Individual, community, and societal perspectives (pp. 41-56). London: Palgrave McMillan.
[18] Delle Fave, A., Bassi, M., & Massimini, F. (2008). Optimal experience and psychological selection. In C. Vasquez, & G. Hervas (Eds.), Positive psychology: Scientific bases of well-being and resilience (pp. 209-230). Madrid: Alianza Editorial.
[19] Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Perigee.
[20] Feldman, R. (2007). On the origins of background emotions: From affect synchrony to symbolic expression. Emotion, 7, 601-611. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.601
[21] Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (1998). Dynamic development of psy chological structures and thought. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 467-561). New York: Wiley.
[22] Fischer, K. W., & Connell, M. W. (2003). The two motivational systems that shape development: Epistemic and self-organizing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph Series II, 2, 102 123.
[23] Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
[24] Fogel, A., de Koeyer, I., Ballagamba, F., & Bell, H. (2002). The dialogical self in the two years of life. Theory & Psychology, 12, 191 205. doi:10.1177/0959354302012002629
[25] Fogel, A., & Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747-761. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.6.747
[26] Gallagher, S. (2003). Bodily self-awareness and object-perception. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum: International Journal for Interdisciplinary Studies, 7, 53-68.
[27] Gallese, V. (2005). Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4, 23-48. doi:10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z
[28] Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Kahlbaugh, P. (1993). Emotion and identity. In M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), The handbook of emotions (pp. 327-339). New York: Guilford.
[29] Hobson, R. P. (1993). The emotional origins of social understanding. Philosophical Psychology, 6, 227-249. doi:10.1080/09515089308573090
[30] Jackson, S. A. (1992). Athletes in flow: A qualitative investigation of flow states in elite figure skaters. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 4, 161-180. doi:10.1080/10413209208406459
[31] Kerr, J. H. (1997). Motivation and emotion in sport. Hove: Psychology Press.
[32] Lambie, J. A., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219-259. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.219
[33] Lavelli, M., Pantoja, A. P. F., Hsu, H., Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2005). Using microgenetic designs to study change processes. In D. M. Teti (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in developmental science (pp. 40-65). Malden, MA: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470756676.ch3
[34] Meltzoff, A. N., & Gallagher, S. (1996). The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology, 9, 211-233. doi:10.1080/09515089608573181
[35] Mounoud, P. (1999). What is homeopathic when you overdose? In Peer commentaries on James Russell’s “Cognitive development as an executive process—in part: A homeopathic dose of Piaget”. Developmental Science, 2, 271-288. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00073
[36] Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184-211. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1
[37] Overton, W. F. (1999). Nativism, empiricism and developmental dynamic action systems. In Peer commentaries on James Russell’s “Cognitive development as an executive process—in part: A homeopathic dose of Piaget”. Developmental Science, 2, 271-288.
[38] Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2000). Perceived self in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 513-530. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00055-8
[39] Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Who’s in the mirror? Self-other discrimination by four and nine-month-old infants. Child Development, 73, 35-46. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00390
[40] Rochat, P. (2003). Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 717-731. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00081-3
[41] Ross, J. (2008). Drawing production, drawing re-experience and drawing re-cognition. In C. Lange-Kuttner, & A. Vinter (Eds.), Drawing and the non-verbal mind (pp.42-62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489730.003
[42] Schmuckler, M. A., & Jewell, D. T. (2006). Infants’ visual-propriocep tive intermodal perception with imperfect contingency information. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 387-398. doi:10.1002/dev.20214
[43] Shaw, R., & Bransford, J. (1977). Perceiving, acting, and knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[44] Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1998). The primacy of movement. Amsterdam: Benjamins P.C.
[45] Stamatopoulou, D. (2004). Integrating the philosophy and psychology of aesthetic experience: Development of aesthetic experience scale. Psychological Reports, 95, 673-695.
[46] Stamatopoulou, D. (2011). Symbol formation and the embodied self: A microgenetic case—Study examination of the transition to stmbolic commumication in scribbling activities from the 14 to 31 months of age. New Ideas in Psychology, 29, 162-188. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.09.001
[47] Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[48] Stern, D. N. (1993). The role of feelings for an interpersonal self. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 205-215). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[49] Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 563-634). New York: Wiley.
[50] Tomasello, M. (1995). Understanding the self as social agent. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in early infancy: Theory and research (pp. 449-460). Amsterdam: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(05)80024-X
[51] Watson, J. S. (1984). Bases of causal inference in infancy: Time, space, and sensory relations. In R. Collier, & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (pp. 152-165). Norwood, NJ: Albex.
[52] Werner, H. (1954). Studies in physiognomic perception: Effects of configurational dynamics and meaning-induced sets on the position of the apparent median plane. Journal of Psychology, 38, 51-65. doi:10.1080/00223980.1954.9712914
[53] Werner, H. (1957). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International University Press.
[54] Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley.
[55] Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 12-17. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.