The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and the MBTI

Abstract

This paper reports on two studies, to attempt to replicate findings that correlated the Myers-Briggs Type Inventory (MBTI) and Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-2). In the first study (n = 514), the clearest correlations indicated that overall Introverted, Sensing and Judging types had lower Emotional Intelligence. The second study (n = 146) indicated that those scoring high on the Thinking-Feeling dimension had higher Emotional Intelligence scores at both the domain and facet levels. Whilst these results confirmed the hypotheses and the findings from previous studies, they also indicated little correlation between the two measures. Implications and limitations are discussed.

Share and Cite:

Furnham, A. (2024) The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and the MBTI. Psychology, 15, 377-392. doi: 10.4236/psych.2024.153023.

1. Introduction

This paper is concerned with the relationship between the Myers-Briggs Type Inventory (MBTI) and Emotional Intelligence (EQ) (Goleman, 1995) . The central question is: what Jungian personality types, as assessed by the MBTI, are most closely associated with the aspects of emotional intelligence? The concept of emotional intelligence is over 30 years old and yet continues to attract a great deal of attention (Furnham, 2009; Furnham & Crump, 2014; Furnham & Rosen, 2016; Furnham et al., 2003; Pérez-Gonzalez et al., 2005; Petrides et al., 2016; Zeidner et al., 2004) . It has been related to many other personality tests, but as far as we know, much less with the MBTI.

1.1. Myers-Briggs Type Inventory (MBTI)

The MBTI is one of the most popular of all personality tests (Furnham, 2020) . Despite its many problems (Pittenger, 2005; Sample, 2017; Sato, 2017) , there has been a great interest in the assessment of EQ since the start of this millennium (Arnau et al., 2003; Bergner et al., 2016; Caplan, 2003; Capraro & Capraro, 2002; Carlson, 1985; Carlyn, 1977; Dawes, 2004; DeVito, 1985; Diekmann et al., 2015; Fretwell et al., 2013; Furnham, 2006; Ginevra et al., 2014; Lloyd, 2022) . There is a continual flow of papers, often in technical journals, reporting how MBTI types behave in a wide variety of work contexts though they are often difficult to access. The following are some of these published over the past decade (Amirhosseini & Kazemian, 2020; Choong & Varathan, 2021; Cohen et al., 2013; Farmer, 2018; Poursafar et al., 2015; Randall et al., 2017; Ross et al., 1996; Sari & Bashori, 2020; Szathmári et al., 2023; Yang et al., 2016; Yoon & Lim, 2018; Zarafshani et al., 2011; Zárate-Torres & Correa, 2023) . These studies testify to the range of correlates of the MBTI that researchers have investigated from construction workers’ safety behaviour to online social forum usage, and attest to the constant interest in the measure.

The MBTI is considered an easy-to-use personality tool and can be utilised for various purposes (Bower, 2015; Lake et al., 2019; Michael, 2003; Murray, 1990) . Annually, over two million, people officially take the MBTI, including employees in 89 of the Fortune 100 companies (Stein & Swan, 2019) . It is also used in a number of academic studies (Kruck et al., 2014; Rushton et al., 2007; Tomat et al., 2021) , particularly in those studying the psychology of religion (Robbins et al., 2010; Ross et al., 1996) . However, it is not the preferred instrument of personality researchers for various psychometric reasons, mainly that it is a typological measure and does not assess essential traits such as Neuroticism and Honesty and Integrity

Myers and McCaulley (1985) developed the original MBTI loosely based on Jung’s theory, which essentially identifies four psychological functions of subjective experience. These functions are encompassed in the MBTI, which yields 16 scores assessed on four dimensions: Extraversion/Introversion (E-I), Sensing/Intuition (S-N), Thinking/Feeling (T-F), and Judging/Perceiving (J-P). The manual suggests that Extraverts relate more easily to the outer world of people and things, while Introverts are engrossed in the inner world of concepts and ideas. Sensing and Intuition are ways of perceiving: Sensing through the five senses and “known facts”, whereas Intuition is more “unconscious”, looking for possibilities and relationships. The two ways of judging are summed up by the opposites of Thinking, which stresses logic and impersonal processes, and Feeling, based more on personal values and judgements. The last dimension is a combination of perception and judgement, with judging types exhibiting preferences for a planned, decided, orderly way of life and the Perceiving type preferring a flexible, spontaneous way of life.

Various studies over many years have looked at the location of the MBTI in the personality factor space. Saggino and Kline (1996) explored correlations between the MBTI and Cattell’s 16PF. McCrae and Costa (1988) and Furnham (1996) investigated the relationship between the MBTI and the Big Five traits. Furnham and Crump (2014) and Furnham (2022) examined the relationship between the MBTI and dark-side traits, while Furnham and McClelland (2022) looked at the relationships between the CPI (California Personality Inventory) and the MBTI. Findings revealed a number of predictable correlations.

Results from many studies suggest the Extraversion-Introversion scale correlates (predictably) highly with other measures of the same construct. Sensing-Intuiting seems to be a unique scale and concept correlated with a few other measures. Thinking-Feeling is associated with poor adjustment, and Judging-Perceiving is loosely associated but weakly with many other traits.

McCrae and Costa (1988) made a number of criticisms of the MBTI, including three clear issues: the fact that it uses a scale (the JP scale), which is not part of Jung’s theory. Also, the measurement identifies people in terms of dominant function, hence, dichotomises preference scores. People are assumed to fit into mutually exclusive groups, yet the scores, when plotted out, are not bi-modally distributed. Further, the MBTI fails to measure Neuroticism, at least openly, despite that all personality researchers have acknowledged this as fundamental to personality description. It seems dated in what it measures and the way traits are assessed.

Stein and Swann (2019) note, “We find that the MBTI theory falters on rigorous theoretical criteria in that it lacks agreement with known facts and data, lacks testability, and possesses internal contradictions” (p. 1). Nevertheless, it is extremely widely used and extensively applied in business settings (Furnham, 2022) . Indeed, it is often used in conjunction with measures of Emotional Intelligence in management training and assessment, because of the popularity of both concepts.

1.2. Emotional Intelligence (EQ)

Goleman’s (1995) book told a simple and fascinating story about emotional intelligence that helped explain its appeal. Technical training is the essential job knowledge of any career and is easier to teach than IQ skills. That is, as an adult, it is comparatively more straightforward to teach a person the technical aspects of the job than the soft skills. The idea is that there is a critical period to acquire the basis of EQ, likely during early to late adolescence. The young person, often a male, may experience social anxiety, discomfort and rejection while attempting to interact with and influence others (specifically those they are attracted to, which is most often people of the opposite sex). Hence, they may find solace in computers and other activities with a high skills/low contact basis. Thus, in early adulthood, some people appear to be technically competent in certain areas (IT, engineering) but remain undeveloped in people skills and, more specifically, emotional awareness and regulation. They may even be “phobic” about emotional issues and resistant to (social skills) training. It is also assumed that people are less able to pick up EQ “skills” and are less willing to try. To acquire technical skills often requires considerable dedication, so opportunities to acquire social skills (EQ) are, therefore, reduced. Then, the low EQ person chooses technology rather than people for fun, comfort, and a source of ideas because they do not understand emotions.

Within the extensive rise in the literature on emotional intelligence, two contrasting approaches have emerged, focusing on emotional intelligence as either a measurable ability (AEI) or a self-reported trait (TEI). TEI is presumed to be represented by behavioral dispositions and self-perceptions of one’s ability to recognize and understand emotions (Petrides & Furnham, 2003; Petrides et al., 2016) . This is essentially the difference between maximal and typical performance, measured by timed and untimed tests.

Further, whilst EI tests have total scores they nearly all have facets. For instance, the TEIQ (Petrides & Furnham, 2001) is divided into four broad categories or factors (Well-being, Self-control, Emotionality and Sociability), formed of 15 different facets and two additional independent ones that provide a more detailed description and understanding of the measurements.

Since the start of the millennium, there have been many studies on EQ (Petrides & Furnham, 2000, 2001, 2003, 2006; Quebbeman & Rozell, 2002) . Two meta-analyses (Joseph & Newman, 2010; O’Boyle et al., 2011) examining the predictive power of EQ on job performance revealed that EQ accounted for unique variance in job performance; that is, it accounts for a portion of any change in job performance. However, there remain questions on how to explain the process and examine possible moderator and mediating factors involved.

1.3. Relationship between the Two Measures

There have been many studies linking certain EQ measures like the TEIQ (Furnham et al., 2003) to personality trait measures, but few to the MBTI. A limited number of studies have indeed examined the relationship between the MBTI and EQ. In an early study, Higgs (2001) tested 177 managers from the UK using the MBTI and the Emotional Intelligence questionnaire devised by Dulewicz and Higgs (1999) . The MBTI Extraversion and Intuition were positively correlated, while Introversion and Sensing negatively correlated with EQ. He argued that Extraversion had the most positive correlations with rudiments of emotional intelligence, including motivation, influence, and intuitive decision-making. Intuition also correlated positively with a large number of emotional intelligence components, such as influence, interpersonal sensitivity, and intuitive decision-making.

Furthermore, Perry and Ball (2005) found a significant correlation between intrapersonal and interpersonal intelligence and the MBTI personality types but no significant relationship with trait EQ. Later, Virmozelova and Dimitrova (2013) revealed that Sensing and Introversion negatively correlated with sharing emotions, empathy and optimism, while Extraversion correlated positively with sharing emotions and empathy. In the same year, Potgieter and Coetzee (2013) tested 304 adults on the MBTI and a measure of employability attributes, which had a subscale measuring EQ (emotional literacy). The only significant correlation was between Extraversion and emotional literacy. However, in an investigation of 81 twelve-year-olds, Séguin and Hipson (2015) used the same EQ measure as used in this study and concluded that Extraversion was positively correlated with many facets of emotional intelligence, and Feeling correlated significantly with the interpersonal variable. Hence, not only is there sparse literature on this topic, but also mixed findings.

Nonetheless, the association between the MBTI and EQ appears to persist across various measures. Dhliwayo and Coetzee (2020) tested 299 Zimbabwean adults on the MBTI and the Wong’s Emotional Intelligence Scale (WEIS) (Wong et al., 2004) , alongside the Assessing Emotions Scale (AES) (Schutte et al., 1998) , which has four subscales (perception of emotion, managing own emotions, managing others’ emotions, and utilisation of emotion). They found the WEIS negatively correlated with the sensing-thinking dichotomy and a significant positive correlation was found between trait EQ (AES overall score) and the MBTI intuition-feeling.

Thus, even when using different measures of EQ in very different populations, the MBTI still yields significant positive correlations between the trait EI Extraversion-Introversion, Sensing-Intuition and Thinking-Feeling (Higgs, 2001; Leary et al., 2009) . However, it should be acknowledged that the effect sizes are small at best, and not all papers replicate findings. We aim to contribute to the growing literature and expand our understanding of these mixed results, with two studies.

2. This Paper

This study uses the EQ-I 2.0, a scale with five domains, each with a number of facets. The first is Self-Perception, which concerns self-awareness. The second is Self-Expression, which encompasses three facets. The third factor is called Interpersonal, with three facets. The fourth factor is Decision-making, which is about planning and completing daily tasks autonomously. The fifth and final factor is Stress Management. We were most interested in the first two domains and how they related to the MBTI. We hypothesise that these EQ scores positively correlate with Extraversion: Intuitive and Feelings scales.

We had access to two data files and therefore attempted to replicate our findings on different population groups but using the same questionnaires.

2.1. Study 1

Method

Participants

There were 514 participants in total, of which 93 were female. They ranged in age from 29 to 57 and were all middle managers and English-speaking.

Measures

1) Myers-Briggs Type Indicator-Form G (MBTI) (Myers & McCalley, 1985) . The Myers-Briggs indicator is a Jungian-based inventory composed of 94 forced-choice items. Respondents are classified into one of 16 personality types based on the largest score obtained for each bipolar scale. The test provides linear scores on each dimension, usually discussed in terms of personality types based on cut-off scores. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator has been the focus of extensive research, and substantial evidence has accumulated, suggesting the inventory has satisfactory concurrent and predictive validity and reliability (Furnham & Stringfield, 1993) .

2) Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (Bar-On, 2011) (EQ-2). Different versions of this measure have been developed to assess the Bar-On model of emotional-social intelligence. The EQ-2 is a self-report measure designed to assess a number of constructs related to EQ. The EQ-2 gives an overall EQ score alongside scores for the five composite scales and 15 subscales (Bar-On, 2004, 2006) . Earlier versions of the measure have been used in many studies (Butler & Chinowsky, 2006; Dawda & Hart, 2000; Ekermans et al., 2011) .

Procedure

Participants were tested by a British-based psychological consultancy. All participants were given personal feedback on their scores. Most were employed as middle to senior managers in European companies.

Study 1 Results

Table 1 depicts the correlational results revealing the expected pattern: the two opposites (i.e. extraversion and introversion) show opposite correlations. The results suggest that the five EQ scores, particularly Intra and Inter-Personal EQ, were strongly positively associated with Extraversion and negatively associated with Intraversion. Correlations for the S-N dimension indicate that N (Intuition) was positively associated with four EQ factors, particularly Stress and Adaptation, but negatively associated with Interpersonal EQ. The T-F dimension implies that T (Thinking) was positively associated with Interpersonal EQ but negatively associated with Stress and Adaptation. The J-P factors show very little relationship, except that Intrapersonal Intelligence was negatively associated with the J (Judging) factor and positively associated with the P (Perceiving) factor.

Table 2 presents the results of a regression of the eight scores onto the Total EQ score. The findings portray very little relationship, with Introversion being the only significant variable.

2.2. Study 2

Participants

There were 146 participants, of which 122 were male. Their age ranged from 31 - 57 years. All were junior and middle managers employed in Great Britain.

Measures

The same as the first study.

Procedure

The same as the first study but data from a different consultancy based in Great Britain.

Table 1. Correlation between the eight MBTI factors, IQ and the five EQ factors.

Note: ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Table 2. Regression of the MBTI onto total EI.

Note: (*)p < .09, *p < .05.

Study 2 Results

Table 3 exhibits that four of six correlations with the T-F dimension were significant, but none with any other dimension were. Four of six correlations were also positively associated with T-F, indicating that those with an F (Feeling) orientation were more likely to have higher EQ.

Table 4 conveys the regressions onto the EQ total and each of the five subscale scores. Results revealed some consistency: while each regression was (marginally) significant, none accounted for more than 8% of the variance. Further, in five of six regressions, the only significant relationship was with the T-F dimension, reinforcing the finding that Feeling types tended to have higher EQ.

3. Discussion

The results of these studies are broadly in agreement with various other studies in the field, which have used very different measures of EQ (Dhliwayo & Coetzee, 2020; Higgs, 2001; Perry & Ball, 2005; Potgieter & Coetzee, 2013; Virmozelova & Dimitrova, 2013) . Essentially, we conclude that people with higher EQ tend to be more Extraverted than Introverted, Intuitive than Sensing, and Feeling than Thought-oriented. In MBTI terms, they are more likely to be ENF type.

However, we make three key observations: First, while several correlations were significant, they were very small and had low effect sizes. Second, although these two studies used identical measures on similar samples, the results were not completely comparable. Third, the second study reveals the T-F dimension to be consistently, but modestly, related to the EQ facet scores.

The different results between the two studies may be a result of a number of different reasons: different sizes and representativeness of the different populations test unreliability and impression management on the part of the participants all partaking in a business assessment setting.

Table 3. Correlations between the four MBTI dimensions, the five scales and total score from the EQ measure.

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 4. (a) Regressions with EQ scores as criterion variables and MBTI as predictor variables; (b) Regressions with EQ scores as criterion variables and MBTI as predictor variables.

Even though few academic studies still use the MBTI to measure personality, it remains a popular instrument among consultants and trainers eager to help people understand their own personality (Furnham, 2018) . One of the most consistent messages of the “gifts differing” philosophy of the MBTI is that all profiles are “equally good” or desirable. The same is not true of more traditional approaches, which measure things such as Neuroticism or Psychoticism. Furnham (2022) found that traits of Narcissism and Paranoia correlated with the Feeling dimension. Similarly, five aberrant traits, particularly Schizoid, Histrionic and Avoidant, were associated with the Perceiving dimensions. Overall, the ten aberrant traits were related very differently to the MBTI traits. However, the MBTI is very rarely used as a “select out” or dark-side detection measure even though extreme scores may indicate evidence of psychopathology.

This study and the literature review suggest a dark side to the Feeling (TF) dimension of the MBTI. The test implies that Thinking and Feeling are essential decision-making (judging) functions. Those with a high thinking score favour all forms of decision-making from a logical, causal, consistent, and rules-based perspective. On the other hand, those favouring Feelings are influenced by issues like harmony, consensus, and fit. The results of various “dark-side” studies suggest that this dimension may be mislabelled and also assesses poor emotional regulation. It is often the case that when professional groups are investigated, there are some “Feeling types” that are considered a problem for the group. However, the data from this and other similar studies suggest that this is not true, instead aligning with the philosophy of the MBTI that all profiles are equally good. It should not be surprising that feeling types are more emotionally sensitive, though this may not always be advantageous in the work setting

Over the years, various studies have related other personality measures, mainly the Big Five to measures of emotional intelligence (Alegre et al., 2019; Petrides et al., 2010; Veselka et al., 2009) . In a recent study, Kumar and Tankha (2023) confirmed the results of many other studies noting that Neuroticism was a negative and most significant predictor of trait EI. This comes as no surprise because emotional regulation is at the heart of Neuroticism/Stability, and it is not measured in the MBTI. This may account, in part for the limited association between the MBTI and EI measures as found in this study.

This study, like all others, had limitations. Most studies comparing two personality tests suffer from method invariance, which often inflates the size of the relationship between the two measures. Furthermore, it would have been desirable to obtain more information regarding the participants, such as their work and relationship status. Finally, it would have been particularly interesting to use different measures of EQ, which though positively correlated, pick up different aspects of EQ.

Data Availability

This is obtainable from the author upon request.

Ethics

This was sought and obtained (CEHP/514/2017).

Informed Consent

Participants gave consent for their anonymised data to be analysed and published.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Alegre, A., Perez-Escoda, N., & Lopez-Cassa, E. (2019). The Relationship between Trait Emotional Intelligence and Personality. Is Trait EI Really Anchored within the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Frameworks? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 866.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00866
[2] Amirhosseini, M. H., & Kazemian, H. (2020). Machine Learning Approach to Personality Type Prediction Based on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator R. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 4, Article 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti4010009
[3] Arnau, R., Green, B., Rosen, D., Gleaves, D., & Melancon, J. (2003). Are Jungian Preferences Really Categorical? An Empirical Investigation Using Taxometric Analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 233-251.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00040-5
[4] Bar-On, R. (2004). The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-I): Rationale, Description and Psychometric Properties. In G. Geher (Ed.), Measuring Emotional Intelligence: Common Ground and Controversy (pp. 115-145). Nova Science.
[5] Bar-On, R. (2006). The Bar-On Model of Emotional-Social Intelligence (ESI). Psicothema, 18, 13-25.
[6] Bar-On, R. (2011). The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-2): Technical Manual. Multi-Health Systems. https://doi.org/10.1037/t04985-000
[7] Bergner, S., Davda, A., Culpin, V., & Rybnicek, R. (2016). Who Overrates, Who Underrates? Personality and Its Link to Self-Other Agreement of Leadership Effectiveness. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 23, 335-354.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1548051815621256
[8] Bower, K. M. (2015). Coaching with the Myers Briggs Type Indicator: A Valuable Tool for Client Self-Awareness. Journal of Practical Consulting, 5, 11-19.
[9] Butler, C. J., & Chinowsky, P. S. (2006). Emotional Intelligence and Leadership Behavior in Construction Executives. Journal of Management in Engineering, 22, 119-125.
https://doi.org/10.1061/(ASCE)0742-597X(2006)22:3(119)
[10] Caplan, B. (2003). Stigler-Becker versus Myers-Briggs: Why Preference-Based Explanations Are Scientifically Meaningful and Empirically Important. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 50, 391-405. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-2681(02)00031-8
[11] Capraro, R., & Capraro, M. (2002). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Score Reliability across Studies: A Meta-Analytic Reliability Generalization Study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 590-602. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164402062004004
[12] Carlson, J. (1985). Recent Assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 356-365. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4904_3
[13] Carlyn, M. (1977). An Assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 461-473. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4105_2
[14] Choong, E. J., & Varathan, K. D. (2021). Predicting Judging-Perceiving of Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) in Online Social Forum. Peer Journal, 23, e11382.
https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.11382
[15] Cohen, Y., Ornoy, H., & Keren, B. (2013). MBTI Personality Types of Project Managers and Their Success: A Field Survey. Project Management Journal, 44, 78-87.
https://doi.org/10.1002/pmj.21338
[16] Dawda, D., & Hart, S. (2000). Assessing Emotional Intelligence: Reliability and Validity of the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-I) in University Students. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 797-812.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00139-7
[17] Dawes, R. (2004). Time for a Critical Empirical Investigation of the MBTI. European Business Forum, 18, 88-89.
[18] DeVito, A. J. (1985). Review of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Ninth Mental Measurements Yearbook, 1, 1030-1032.
[19] Dhliwayo, P., & Coetzee, M. (2020). Cognitive Intelligence, Emotional Intelligence and Personality Types as Predictors of Job Performance: Exploring a Model for Personnel Selection. SA Journal of Human Resource Management, 18, a1348.
https://doi.org/10.4102/sajhrm.v18i0.1348
[20] Diekmann, J., König, C. J., & Alles, J. (2015). The Role of Neuroscience Information in Choosing a Personality Test: Not as Seductive as Expected. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 23, 99-108. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijsa.12099
[21] Dulewicz, S. V., & Higgs, M. J. (1999). Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Manual and Users’ Guide. ASE/NFER-Nelson.
[22] Ekermans, G., Saklofske, D., Austin, E., & Stough, C. (2011). Measurement Invariance and Differential Item Functioning of the Bar-On EQ-I: S Measure over Canadian, Scottish, South African and Australia Samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 286-290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.004
[23] Farmer, A. (2018). Leading Like an Educator: How MBTI Profiles Vary from the Norm. Journal of Global Education and Research, 2, 60-67.
https://doi.org/10.5038/2577-509X.2.1.1041
[24] Fretwell, C., Lewis, C., & Hanney, M. (2013). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, A/B Personality Types, and Locus of Control: Where Do They Intersect? American Journal of Management, 13, 57-66.
[25] Furnham, A. (2009). Sex, IQ, and Emotional Intelligence. Psychological Reports, 105, 1092-1094. https://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.105.F.1092-1094
[26] Furnham, A. (1996). The Big Five versus the Big Four: The Relationship between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and the NEO-PI Five Factor Model of Personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 303-307.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00033-5
[27] Furnham, A. (2006). Explaining the Popularity of Emotional Intelligence. In K. R. Murphy (Ed.), A Critique of Emotional Intelligence (pp. 141-159). Psychology Press.
[28] Furnham, A. (2018). The Great Divide: Academic versus Practitioner Criteria for Psychometric Test Choice. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100, 498-506.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2018.1488134
[29] Furnham, A. (2020). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). In V. Zeigler-Hill, & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of Personality and Individual Differences (pp. 3059-3062). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_50
[30] Furnham, A. (2022). MBTI and Aberrant Personality Traits: Dark-Side Trait Correlates of the Myers Briggs Type Inventory. Psychology, 13, 805-815.
https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2022.135054
[31] Furnham, A., & Crump, J. (2014). The Dark Side of the MBTI. Psychology, 5, 166-171.
https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.52026
[32] Furnham, A., & McClelland, M. (2022). Folk Concepts and Jung: The Relationship between the California Personality Inventory (CPI) and the MBTI. Psychology, 13, 829-841.
https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2022.135056
[33] Furnham, A., & Rosen, A. (2016). The Dark Side of Emotional Intelligence. Psychology, 7, 326-334. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2016.73035
[34] Furnham, A., & Stringfield, P. (1993). Personality and Occupational Behavior: Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Correlates of Managerial Practices in Two Cultures. Human Relations, 46, 827-848. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872679304600703
[35] Furnham, A., Moutafi, J., & Crump, J. (2003). The Relationship between the Revised NEO-Personality Inventory and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 31, 577-584.
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2003.31.6.577
[36] Ginevra, M. C., Nota, L., Heppner, P. P., Heppner, M., & Soresi, S. (2014). The Relationship of Personality Type, Problem Solving Appraisal, and Learning Strategies. Journal of Career Assessment, 23, 545-558. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072714553075
[37] Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence: Why It Can Matter More Than IQ. Bloomsbury.
[38] Higgs, M. (2001). Is There a Relationship between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and Emotional Intelligence? Journal of Managerial Psychology, 16, 509-533.
https://doi.org/10.1108/EUM0000000006165
[39] Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional Intelligence: An Integrative Meta-Analysis and Cascading Model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 54-78.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286
[40] Kruck, S. E., Sendall, P., Ceccucci, W., Peslak, A., & Hunsinger, S. (2014). Does Personality Play a Role in Computer Information Systems Performance? Issues in Information Systems, 15, 383-392.
[41] Kumar, V., & Tankha, G. (2023). Association between the Big Five and Trait Emotional Intelligence among College Students, Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 16, 915-925. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S400058
[42] Lake, C. J., Carlson, J., Rose, A., & Chlevin-Thiele, C. (2019). Trust in Name Brand Assessments: The Case of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. The Psychologist-Manager Journal, 22, 91-107. https://doi.org/10.1037/mgr0000086
[43] Leary, M. M., Reilly, M. D., & Brown, F. W. (2009). A Study of Personality Preferences and Emotional Intelligence. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 30, 421-434.
https://doi.org/10.1108/01437730910968697
[44] Lloyd, J. B. (2022). Seeking Truth in Personality Science: Reconciling Trait Theory and Psychological Type. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 25, 817-828.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2022.2158794
[45] McCrae, R., & Costa, P. (1988). Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator from the Perspective of the Five-Factor Model of Personality. Journal of Personality, 57, 17-40.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00759.x
[46] Michael, J. (2003). Using the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator as a Tool for Leadership Development? Apply with Caution. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 10, 68-81. https://doi.org/10.1177/107179190301000106
[47] Murray, J. (1990). Review of Research on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1187-1202. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1990.70.3c.1187
[48] Myers, I., & McCaulley, M. (1985). Manual: A Guide to the Development and Use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Consulting Psychologists.
[49] O’Boyle, E. H., Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., Hawver, T. H., & Story, P. A. (2011). The Relation between Emotional Intelligence and Job Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 788-818. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.714
[50] Pérez-Gonzalez, J. C., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring Trait Emotional Intelligence. In R. Schulze, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Emotional Intelligence: An International Handbook (pp. 123-143). Hogrefe & Huber.
[51] Perry, C., & Ball, I. (2005). Emotional Intelligence and Teaching: Further Validation Evidence. Issues in Educational Research, 15, 175-192.
[52] Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2000). On the Dimensional Structure of Emotional Intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 313-320.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00195-6
[53] Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait Emotional Intelligence: Psychometric Investigation with Reference to Established Trait Taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 425-448. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.416
[54] Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait Emotional Intelligence: Behavioural Validation in Two Studies of Emotion Recognition and Reactivity to Mood Induction. European Journal of Personality, 17, 39-57. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.466
[55] Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). The Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence in a Gender-Specific Model of Organizational Variables. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 552-569. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00019.x
[56] Petrides, K. V., Siegling, A. B., & Saklofske, D. H. (2016). Theory and Measurement of Trait Emotional Intelligence. In U. Kumar (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment (pp. 90-103). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119173489.ch7
[57] Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., Ligthart, L., Boomsma, D. I., & Veselka, L. (2010). Relationships between Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Big Five in the Netherlands. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 906-910.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.019
[58] Pittenger, D. J. (2005). Cautionary Comments Regarding the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 57, 210-221.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1065-9293.57.3.210
[59] Potgieter, I., & Coetzee, M. (2013). Employability Attributes and Personality Preferences of Postgraduate Business Management Students. South African Journal of Industrial Psychology, 39, Article No. 1064. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajip.v39i1.1064
[60] Poursafar, Z., Devi, N., & Rodrigues, L. (2015). Evaluation of Myers-Briggs Personality Traits in Offices and Its Effects on Productivity of Employees: An Empirical Study. International Journal of Current Research and Review, 7, 53-58.
[61] Quebbeman, A. J., & Rozell, E. J. (2002). Emotional Intelligence and Dispositional Affectivity as Moderators of Workplace Aggression: The Impact on Behavior Choice. Human Resource Management Review, 12, 125-143.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-4822(01)00054-7
[62] Randall, K., Isaacson, M., & Ciro, C. (2017). Validity and Reliability of the Myers-Briggs Personality Type Indicator: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Best Practices in Health Professions Diversity, 10, 1-27.
[63] Robbins, M., Francis, L. J., & Burton, L. (2010). Psychological Type Profile of Methodist Circuit Ministers in Britain: Similarities to and Differences from Anglican Clergy. Journal of Empirical Theology, 23, 64-81. https://doi.org/10.1163/157092510X503020
[64] Ross, C. F. J., Weiss, D., & Jackson, L. M. (1996). The Relation of Jungian Psychological Type to Religious Attitudes and Practices. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 6, 263-279. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327582ijpr0604_3
[65] Rushton, S., Morgan, J., & Richard, M. (2007). Teacher’s Myers-Briggs Personality Profiles: Identifying Effective Teacher Personality Traits. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 432-441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2006.12.011
[66] Saggino, A., & Kline, P. (1996). The Location of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator in Personality Factor Space. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 591-597.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00009-8
[67] Sample, J. (2017). A Review of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator in Public Affairs Education. Journal of Public Affairs Education, 23, 979-992.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15236803.2017.12002300
[68] Sari, E. Y., & Bashori, K. (2020). Predicting Managerial Styles: Is the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Still Useful? Journal of Education and Learning, 14, 617-622.
https://doi.org/10.11591/edulearn.v14i4.14582
[69] Sato, J. (2017). Additional Report about the Validity of the Jung Psychological Types Scale. Online Journal of Japanese Clinical Psychology, 4, 1-7.
[70] Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., & Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and Validation of a Measure of Emotional Intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4
[71] Séguin, D. G., & Hipson, W. (2015). Unravelling the Complex Associations between Emotional Intelligence and Personality in Later Childhood and Early Adolescence. Early Child Development and Care, 186, 1212-1226.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2015.1084803
[72] Stein, R., & Swan, A. B. (2019). Evaluating the Validity of Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Theory: A Teaching Tool and Window into Intuitive Psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13, e12434. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12434
[73] Szathmári, E., Czibor, A., Bents, R., Szabó, Z. P., & Kiss, O. E. (2023). Jungian Personality Type Preferences of Female and Male Hungarian Leaders. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article 1222568. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1222568
[74] Tomat, L., Trkman, P., & Manfreda, A. (2021). Personality in Information Systems Professions: Identifying Archetypal Professions with Suitable Traits and Candidates’ Ability to Fake-Good These Traits. Information Technology & People, 35, 52-73.
https://doi.org/10.1108/ITP-03-2021-0212
[75] Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., Petrides, K. V., Cherkas, L. F., Spector, T. D., & Vernon P. A. (2009). A General Factor of Personality: Evidence from the HEXACO Model and a Measure of Trait Emotional Intelligence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 420-424.
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.12.5.420
[76] Virmozelova, N., & Dimitrova, N. (2013). Relationship between Personality Types Conceptualized by C. G. Jung and Emotional Intelligence. Psychological Thought, 6, 339-357.
https://doi.org/10.5964/psyct.v6i2.83
[77] Wong, C. S., Law, K. S., & Wong, P. (2004). Developing and Validation of a Forced Choice Emotional Intelligence Measure for Chinese Respondents in Hong Kong. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 21, 535-559.
https://doi.org/10.1023/B:APJM.0000048717.31261.d0
[78] Yang, C, Richard, G., & Durkin, M. (2016). The Association between Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and Psychiatry as the Specialty Choice. International Journal of Medical Education, 7, 48-51. https://doi.org/10.5116/ijme.5698.e2cd
[79] Yoon, C. H., & Lim, D. S. (2018). The Effect of the Big Five and the MBTI on Impulsive and Compulsive Buying Behaviors: An Integrated Analysis in Online Shopping. Journal of International Trade & Commerce, 14, 101-117.
https://doi.org/10.16980/jitc.14.3.201806.101
[80] Zarafshani, K., Sharafi, L., & Rajabi, S. (2011). Using the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) in the Teaching of Entrepreneurial Skills. International Journal of Science and Technology Education Research, 2, 67-74.
[81] Zárate-Torres, R., & Correa, J. C. (2023). How Good Is the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator for Predicting Leadership-Related Behaviors? Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article 940961.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.940961
[82] Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., & Roberts, R. D. (2004). Emotional Intelligence in the Workplace: A Critical Review. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53, 371-399.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00176.x

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.