Investigating Cognitive Processes Underlying Reading in Arabic: Evidence from Typical and Poor Reading Performance

Abstract

The current study has examined by which cognitive factors at the field of phonological and visual processing, rapid automatized naming and morphological awareness, contribute to reading and decoding abilities among typical and poor native Arabic readers. In the current study, the performances of two groups of participants, typical and poor readers were compared through tasks that examined their performance with regard to the areas of functions mentioned above in addition to other tasks that examined their performance in reading meaningful and pseudo words. The study’s results have indicated that the visual and phonological processing skills play an important role in reading among typical and poor readers as well. Moreover, it was found that the visual and phonological processing skills of participants with poor reading skills were lower in comparison to participants with typical reading abilities.

Share and Cite:

Taha, H. (2013). Investigating Cognitive Processes Underlying Reading in Arabic: Evidence from Typical and Poor Reading Performance. Psychology, 4, 1018-1026. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.412148.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Abu-Rabia, S. (1997a). Reading in Arabic orthography: The effect of vowels and context on reading accuracy of poor and skilled native Arabic readers in reading paragraphs, sentences, and isolated words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 465-482.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025034220924
[2] Abu-Rabia, S. (1997b). Reading in Arabic orthography: The effect of vowels and context on reading accuracy of poor and skilled native Arabic readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 65-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1007962408827
[3] Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading Semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 39-59.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008147606320
[4] Abu-Rabia, S. (2007). The role of morphology and short vowelization in reading Arabic of normal and dyslexic readers in grades 3, 6, 9, and 12. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 89-106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10936-006-9035-6
[5] Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and spelling error analysis of native Arabic dyslexic readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 651-689.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-2657-x
[6] Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2005). Reading in Arabic orthography: Characteristics, research findings, and assessment. In R. M. Joshi, & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 321-338). New York: Routledge.
[7] Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2006). Phonological errors predominate in Arabic spelling across grades 1-9. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 167-188.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10936-005-9010-7
[8] Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 423-442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024237415143
[9] Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[10] Ayari, S. (1996). Diglossia and illiteracy in the Arab world. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 9, 243-253.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07908319609525233
[11] Beery, E. K. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual motor integration with supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination. New Jersey: Modern Curriculum Press.
[12] Ben-Dror, I., Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1995). Semantic, phonologic, and morphologic skills in reading disabled and normal children. Reading Research Quarterly, 30(4), 876-893.
http://www.jstor.org/stable/748202
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/748202
[13] Bowers, P. G., & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition. Reading and Writing, 15, 109-126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1013820421199
[14] Bradly, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746-747. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/271746a0
[15] Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[16] Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.) Morphological aspects of language processing (pp 189-209). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[17] Clark, E. V., & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition, 12, 1-24.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(82)90027-0
[18] Coltheart, M. (2005). Modelling reading: The dual-route approach. In: M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The Science of Reading. Oxford: Blackwells Publishing. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642
[19] Denckla, M. B., & Cutting, L. E. (1999). History and significance of rapid automatized naming. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 29-42.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-999-0018-9
[20] Denkla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid automatized naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186-202.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(74)80009-2
[21] Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167-188.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4
[22] Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of Morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209-240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02648177
[23] Everatt, J. (1999). Reading and dyslexia: Visual and attentional processes. New York: Routledge.
[24] Everatt, J., McCorquodale, B., Smith, J., culverwell, F., wilks, A., Evans, D., Kay, M., & Baker, D. (1999). Association between reading ability and visual processes. In: Everatt, J (Ed.), Reading and dyslexia: Visual and attentional processes (pp 1-39). New York: Routledge.
[25] Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109-123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70840-2
[26] Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 14, 47-56.
[27] Friedmann, N., & Gvion, A. (2001). Letter position dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 673-696.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290143000051
[28] Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In: Patterson, K. E., Marashall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia. (pp 301-330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[29] Glickstein, M. (2000). How are visual areas of the brain connected to motor areas for the sensory guidance of movement? Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 613-617.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01681-7
[30] Goodale M, A., & Milner A, D. (2004). Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. New York: Oxford University Press.
[31] Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Erlbaum.
[32] Heiervang, E., & Hugdahl, K. (2003). Impaired visual attention in children with dyslexia. Journal of learning disabilities, 36, 68-73.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194030360010801
[33] Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of the Arabic orthography slow it’s cognitive processing. Neuropsycholgy, 16, 322-326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.322
[34] Jaækowski, P., & Rusiak, P. (2005). Posterior parietal cortex and developmental dyslexia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis (Wars), 65, 79-94.
[35] Khateb, A., Khateb, M., Taha, H., & Ibrahim, R. (Submitted). The effect of the internal connectedness of written Arabic words on the process of the visual recognition: A developmental study.
[36] Kinsey, K., Rose, M., Hansen, P., Richardson, A., & Stein, J. (2004). Magnocellular mediated visual-spatial attention and reading ability. Neuroreport, 15, 2215-2218.
[37] Levine, I., Ravid, D., & Rapaport, S. (2001). Morphology and spelling among Hebrew-speaking children: From kindergarten to first grade. Journal of Child Language, 28, 741-772.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000901004834
[38] Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159-173.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1980.tb00356.x
[39] Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[40] Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (2006). The effects of learning to spell on children’s awareness of morphology. Reading and writing, 19, 767-787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9025-y
[41] Ravid, D. (2001). Learning to spell in Hebrew: Phonological and morphological factors. Reading and Writing, 14, 459-485.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1011192806656
[42] Rubin, H. (1991). Morphological knowledge and writing ability. In R. M. Joshi (Ed.), Written Language Disorders (pp. 43-69). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
[43] Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., & Broman, M. (1978). Rapid silent response to repeated target symbols by dyslexic and nondyslexic children. Brain and Language, 6, 52-62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(78)90043-3
[44] Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 431-451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000225
[45] Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of word and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 495-512.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001249
[46] Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21, 481-504.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9074-x
[47] Senechal, M. (2000). Morphological effects in children’s spelling of French words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 76-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087331
[48] Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
[49] Snowling, M. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 37-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.185
[50] Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., & Defty, N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 653-669.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.653
[51] Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-406.
http://www.jstor.org/stable/747612
[52] Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590-604.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221948802101003
[53] Taha, H. Y. (2013). Reading and spelling in Arabic: Linguistic and orthographic complexity. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 3, 721-727. http://dx.doi.org/10.4304/tpls.3.5.721-727
[54] Taha, H., Ibrahim, R., & Khateb, A. (2013). How does Arabic orthographic connectivity modulate brain activity during visual word recognition: An ERP study. Brain Topography, 26, 292-302.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10548-012-0241-2
[55] Taha, H., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (Submitted). Morphology and spelling in Arabic: Development and interface.
[56] Tong, X. L., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 523-534.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023495
[57] Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.
[58] Treiman, R., & Bourassa, D. C. (2000). The development of spelling skills. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 1-18.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00011363-200020030-00004
[59] Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the role of the magnocellular pathway in attention. Neuroreport, 10, 1283-1287.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199904260-00024
[60] Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192
[61] Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415
[62] Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G. & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940003300409
[63] Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
[64] Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.