Differential Sensibility of Information Processing Capacity with Age: Effects of Physical Activity and Task Complexity

Abstract

Study aim: Movement control systems are altered by the aging process. Numerous researches have explained the changes that occur with aging, and many of those changes are related to central nervous system (CNS) effects. This article evaluates the impact of age, the practice of regular physical activity, and the task complexity on decision-making ability. Methods: 120 healthy male subjects volunteered to participate in this study. They included 60 young adults (i.e., 30 sedentary and 30 active) (age: 24.35 ± 2.82 years), as well as 60 older adults (age: 66.42 ± 4.06 years) (i.e., 30 sedentary and 30 active). They performed two types of tasks (i.e., simple and complex) to measure reaction time (RT). Subjects perceive visual stimuli through the computer screen. Results: Our results showed that older active subjects have lower RT than older sedentary subjects (p < 0.05). However, no significant difference was observed in young adults. Moreover, young adults had significantly lower RT than older subjects (p < 0.05). Besides, we observed a significant increase in the RT when task is complex compared to the simple task in all groups. In addition, active adults have better RT regardless the complexity of the task (i.e., simple or complex). Conclusions: Physical activity improves the decision making ability in older subjects.

Share and Cite:

K. Souha, A. Salma, G. Youssef and B. Jean, "Differential Sensibility of Information Processing Capacity with Age: Effects of Physical Activity and Task Complexity," Open Journal of Medical Psychology, Vol. 2 No. 1, 2013, pp. 1-6. doi: 10.4236/ojmp.2013.21001.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] S. Resnick, D. Pham, A. Kraut, A. Zonderman and C. Davatzikos, “Longitudinal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Older Adults: A Shrinking Brain,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 23, No. 8, 2003, pp. 3295-3301.
[2] N. Raz, U. Lindenberger, K. Rodrigue, K. Kennedy, D. Head, A. Williamson, C. Dahle, D. Gerstorf and J. Acker, “Regional Brain Changes in Aging Healthy Adults: General Trends, Individual Differences and Modifiers,” Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 15, No. 11, 2005, pp. 1676-1689. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi044
[3] I. Driscoll, C. Davatzikos, Y. An, X. Wu, D. Shen, M. Kraut and S. Resnick, “Longitudinal Pattern of Regional Brain Volume Change Differentiates Normal Aging from MCI,” Neurology, Vol. 72, No. 22, 2009, pp. 1906-1913. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a82634
[4] A. Fjell, K.Walhovd, C. Fennema-Notestine, L. Mcevoy, D. Hagler, D. Holland, J. Brewer and A. Dale, “One-Year Brain Atrophy Evident in Healthy Aging,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 29, No. 27, 2009, pp. 15223-15231. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0115-09.2009
[5] G. Bartzokis, J. Cummings, D. Sultzer, V. Henderson, K. Nuechterlein and J. Mintz, “White Matter Structural Integrity in Healthy Aging Adults and Patients with Alzheimer Disease: A Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study,” Archives of Neurology, Vol. 60, No. 3, 2003, pp. 393-398. doi:10.1001/archneur.60.3.393
[6] F. Gunning-Dixon, A. Brickman, J. Cheng and G. Alexopoulos, “Aging of Cerebral White Matter: A Review of MRI Findings,” International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, Vol. 24, No. 2, 2009, pp. 109-117. doi:10.1002/gps.2087
[7] C. Morse, “Does Variability Increase with Age? An Archival Study of Cognitive Measures,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 8, No. 2, 1993, pp. 156-164. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.8.2.156
[8] R. Wilson, L. Beckett, L. Barnes, J. Schneider, J. Bach, D. Evans and D. Bennett, “Individual Differences in Rates of Change in Cognitive Abilities of Older Persons,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 17, No. 2, 2002, pp. 179-193. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.179
[9] C. Van Petten, E. Plante, P. Davidson, T. Kuo, L. Bajuscak and E. Glisky, “Memory and Executive Function in Older Adults: Relationships with Temporal and Prefrontal Gray Matter Volumes and White Matter Hyperintensties,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 42, No. 10, 2004, pp. 1313-1335. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.009
[10] R. Verrillo, “Effects of Aging on the Suprathreshold Responses to Vibration,” Perception & Psychophysics, Vol. 32, No. 1, 1982, pp 61-68. doi:10.3758/BF03204869
[11] G. Gescheider, S. Bolanowski, K. Hall, K. Hoffman and R. Verrillo, “The Effects of Aging on Information-Processing Channels in the Sense of Touch: Ⅰ. Absolute Sensitivity,” Somatosensory and Motor Research, Vol. 11, No. 4, 1994, pp. 345-357. doi:10.3109/08990229409028879
[12] R. Verrillo, S. Bolanowski, G. Gescheider, “Effect of Aging on the Subjective Magnitude of Vibration,” Somatosensory and Motor Research, Vol. 19, No. 3, 2002, pp. 238-244. doi:10.1080/0899022021000009161
[13] Y. Lin, S. Hsieh, C. Chao, Y. Chang and S. Hsieh, “Influence of Aging on Thermal and Vibratory Thresholds of Quantitative Sensory Testing,” Journal of Peripheral Nervous System, Vol. 10, No. 3, 2005, pp. 269-281. doi:10.1111/j.1085-9489.2005.10305.x
[14] H. Dinse, N. Kleibel, T. Kalisch, P. Ragert, C. Wilimzig and M. Tegenthoff, “Tactile Coactivation Resets Age-Related Decline of Human Tactile Discrimination,” Annals of Neurology, Vol. 60, No. 1, 2006, pp. 88-94. doi:10.1002/ana.20862
[15] P. Greenwood, “Functional Plasticity in Cognitive Aging: Review and Hypothesis,” Neuropsychology, Vol. 21, No. 6, 2007, pp. 657-673. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.680
[16] P. Greenwood and R. Parasuraman, “Neuronal and Cognitive Plasticity: A Neurocognitive Framework for Ameliorating Cognitive Aging,” Frontiers in Aging Neurosciene, Vol. 2, No. 150, 2010. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2010.00150
[17] P. Lemaire and L. Bherer, “Psychologie du Vieillissement. Une Approche Cognitive,” De Boeck Université, Bruxelles, 2005.
[18] D. Park and A. H. Gutchess, “Aging, Cognition and Culture: A Neuroscientific Perspective,” Neurosciences Biobehavioral Review, Vol. 26, No. 7, 2002, pp. 859-867.
[19] L. Clarkson-Smith and A. Hartley, “Relationships between Physical Exercise and Cognitive Abilities in Older Adults,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 4, No. 2, 1989, pp. 183-189. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.4.2.183
[20] K. Yaffe, D. Barnes, M. Nevitt, L. Lui and K. Covinsky, “A Prospective Study of Physical Activity and Cognitive Decline in Elderly Women-Women Who Walk,” Archives of Internal Medicine, Vol. 161, No. 14, 2001, pp. 1703-1708. doi:10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703
[21] C. Emery and M. Gatz, “Psychological and Cognitive Effects of an Exercise Program for Community-Residing Older Adults,” The Gerontologist, Vol. 30, No. 2, 1990, pp. 184-188. doi:10.1093/geront/30.2.184
[22] L. Clarkson-Smith and A. Hartley, “Structural Equation Models of Relationships between Exercise and Cognitive Abilities,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 5, No. 3, 1990, pp. 437-446.
[23] D. Bunce, “The Locus of Age X Health-Related Physical Fitness Interactions in Serial Choice Responding as a function of Task Complexity, Central processing or Motor Function?” Experimental Aging Research, Vol. 27, No. 1, 2001, pp. 103-122.
[24] J. Etnier and D. Landers, “Motor Performance and Motor Learning as a Function of Age and Fitness,” Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Vol. 69, No. 2, 1998, pp. 136-146.
[25] J. L. Etnier, D. H. Romero and T. Traustadottir, “Acquisition and Retention of Motor Skills as a Function of Age and Aerobic Fitness,” Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, Vol. 9, No. 4, 2001, pp. 425-437.
[26] W. Spirduso, H. MacRae, P. MacRae, J. Prewitt and L. Osborne, “Exercise Effects on Aged Motor Function,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 515, No. 1, 1988, pp. 363-375.
[27] A. Kriska, W. Knowler, R. LaPorte, A. Drash, R. Wing, S. Blair, P. Bennett and L. Kuller, “Development of Questionnaire to Examine Relationship of Physical Activity and Diabetes in Pima Indians,” Diabetes Care, Vol. 13, No. 4, 1990, pp. 401-411. doi:10.2337/diacare.13.4.401
[28] B. Ainsworth, W. Haskell, A. Leon, D. Jacobs, H. Montoye, J. Sallis and R. Paffenbarger, “Compendium of Physical Activity: Classification of Energy Costs of Human Physical Activities,” Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, Vol. 25, No. 1, 1993, pp. 71-80 doi:10.1249/00005768-199301000-00011
[29] E. Fox and T. Mathews, “Bases Physiologiques de l’Exercice Physique,” Vigot, Paris, 1984.
[30] C. Ketcham and G. Stelmach, “Age-Related Declines in Motor Control,” In: J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie, Eds., Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, Academic Press, San Diego, 2001, pp. 313-348.
[31] J. Fozard, M. Vercryssen, S. Reynolds, P. Hancock and R. Quilter, “Age Differences and Changes in Reaction Time: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging,” Journal of Gerontology, Vol. 49, No. 4, 1994, pp.179-189. doi:10.1093/geronj/49.4.P179
[32] R. Ratcliff, A. Thapar and G. Mckoon, “The Effects of Aging on Reaction Time in a Signal Detection Task,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 16, No. 2, 2001, pp. 323-341. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.323
[33] S. Colcombe and A. Kramer, “Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study,” Psychological science, Vol. 14, No. 2, 2003, pp. 125-130. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430
[34] T. Salthouse and K. Prill, “Inferences about Age Impairments in Inferential Reasoning,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 2, No. 1, 1987, pp. 43-51. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.2.1.43
[35] W. Spirduso, “Physical Dimensions of Aging,” 2nd Edtion, Human Kinetics, Leeds, 2005.
[36] W. Spirduso, “Physical Fitness, Aging and Psychomotor Speed: A Review,” Journal of Gerontology, Vol. 35, No. 6, 1980, pp. 850-865. doi:10.1093/geronj/35.6.850
[37] M. Stones and A. Kozma, “Physical Activity, Age and Cognitive/Motor Performance,” In: L. Howe and C. J. Brainerd, Eds., Cognitive Development in Adulthood, Springer Verlag, New York, 1988, pp. 271-321. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-3852-2_9
[38] J. Cerella, L. Poon and D. Williams, “Age and the Complexity Hypothesis,” In: L. W. Poon, Ed., Aging in the 1980, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1980, pp. 332-340. doi:10.1037/10050-024
[39] J. Fisk and C. Sharp, “Syllogistic Reasoning and Cognitive Ageing,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 55, No. 4, 2002, pp. 1273-1293. doi:10.1080/02724980244000107
[40] A. Gilinsky and B. Judd, “Working Memory and Bias in Reasoning across the Life Span,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 9, No. 3, 1994, pp. 356-371. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.356
[41] T. Abourezk and T. Toole, “Effect of Task Complexity on the Relationship between Physical Fitness and Reaction Time in Older Women,” Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, Vol. 3, No. 3, 1995, pp. 251-260.
[42] A. Baylor and W. Spirduso, “Systematic Aerobic Exercise and Components of Reaction Time in Older Women,” Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, Vol. 43, No. 5, 1988, pp. 121-126.
[43] T. Toole, S. Park and H. Al Ameer, “Years of Physical Activity Can Affect Simple and Complex Cognitive/Motor Speed in Older Adults,” General Electric, Fairfield, 1993.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.