The Pale Shades of Emotion: A Signal Detection Theory Analysis of the Emotional Stroop Task

Abstract

In the emotional Stroop effect (ESE), people are slower to name the ink color of negative, emotion-laden words than that of neutral words. Two accounts have been suggested for the ESE, relating it to either deficient attention to color or to temporary disruption of action in the face of threat. Is the ESE driven by a threat-produced change in perception, or is it a strategic bias in responding? In a pioneer import of Signal Detection Theory to this realm, threat was found to diminish the psychological distance (d’) between the ink colors, but it did not impact response bias (β). The results indicate that the ESE derives from a deep perceptual change engendered by the negative stimuli and not from changes in the criterion for responding. These results constrain future theorizing in the domain of emotion-produced changes in behavior, and provide some support for the threat account of attention under emotion.

Share and Cite:

Ben-David, B. , Chajut, E. & Algom, D. (2012). The Pale Shades of Emotion: A Signal Detection Theory Analysis of the Emotional Stroop Task. Psychology, 3, 537-541. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.37079.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 323-338. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323
[2] Algom, D., Zakay, D., Monar, O., & Chajut, E. (2009). Wheel chairs and arm chairs: A novel experimental design for the emotional Stroop effect. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1552-1564. doi:10.1080/02699930802490243
[3] Ben-David, B. M., Nguyen, L. L. T., & Van Lieshout, P. H. H. M. (2011). Stroop effects in persons with traumatic brain injury: Selective attention, speed of processing or color-vision? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 354-363. doi:10.1017/S135561771000175X
[4] Ben-David, B. M., & Schneider, B. A. (2009). A sensory origin for color-word Stroop effects in aging: A Meta-Analysis. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 16, 505-534. doi:10.1080/13825580902855862
[5] Ben-David, B. M., & Schneider, B. A. (2010). A sensory origin for color-word Stroop effects in aging: Simulating age-related changes in color-vision mimics age-related changes in Stroop. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17, 730-746. doi:10.1080/13825585.2010.510553
[6] Ben-David, B. M., Van Lieshout, P. H. H. M., & Leszcz, T. (2011). A resource of validated affective and neutral sentences to assess identification of emotion in spoken language after a brain injury. Brain Injury, 25, 206-220. doi:10.3109/02699052.2010.536197
[7] Chajut, E., Mama, Y., Levi, L., & Algom, D. (2010). Avoiding the approach trap: A response bias theory of the emotional Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1567-1572. doi:10.1037/a0020710
[8] Chajut, E., Schupak, A., & Algom, D. (2010). Emotional dilution of the Stroop effect: A new tool for assessing attention under emotion. Emotion, 10, 944-948. doi:10.1037/a0020172
[9] Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.681
[10] Frings, C., Englert, J., Wentura, D., & Bermeitinger, C. (2010). Decomposing the emotional Stroop effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 42-49. doi:10.1080/17470210903156594
[11] Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 219-234. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.2.219
[12] Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. (2005). Detection theory. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[13] Marks, L. E., & Algom, D. (1998). Psychophysical scaling. In M. H. Birnbaum (Eds.), Measurement, judgment, and decision making. Handbook of perception and cognition (pp. 81-178). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
[14] McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: An investigation of the emotional Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1595-1607. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.6.1595
[15] McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: The role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 382-392. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382
[16] ?hman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466-478. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.466
[17] Schupp, H. T., Junghoefer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2003). Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychological Science, 14, 7-13. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.01411
[18] Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97-108. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb01985.x
[19] Westermann, S., & Lincoln, T. M. (2010). Using signal detection theory to test the impact of negative emotion on sub-clinical paranoia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 96- 101. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.10.007
[20] Wiens, S., Peira, N., Golkar, A., & ?hman, A. (2008). Recognizing masked threat: Fear betrays, but disgust you can trust. Emotion, 8, 810-819. doi:10.1037/a0013731
[21] Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
[22] Windmann, S., & Chmielewski, A. (2008). Emotion-induced modulation of recognition memory decisions in a Go/NoGo task: Response bias or memory bias? Cognition and Emotion, 22, 761-776. doi:10.1080/02699930701507899
[23] Windmann, S., & Krüger, T. (1998). Subconscious detection of threat as re?ected by an enhanced response bias. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 603-633. doi:10.1006/ccog.1998.0337
[24] Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Rotteveel, M. (2006). The impact of emotion on perception: Bias or enhanced processing? Psychological Science, 17, 287-291. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01700.x

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.