Physical and Behavioral Markers Help Identify Written Language Disability (WLD) Related to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
Rowe A. Young, Benson E. Ginsburg, Dawn Bradway
.
DOI: 10.4236/psych.2012.31006   PDF    HTML     7,953 Downloads   17,326 Views   Citations

Abstract

Rotation, its physiological processing, as well as how this lateralized direction of movement is sensed, is regularly taken for granted and little appreciated when studying how learning develops. Preference for direction of rotational movement and how one processes this information has been found to differ greatly from one individual to another. This report discusses how bimanual rotational hand use preference and reversed direction of body part and movement sensation detection may be utilized to help identify certain currently unrecognized classroom learning difficulties. What we label Reversed Positioning Sensation (RPS) is evidenced in our research and is found in many children and adults presently identified as ‘learning disabled’ and/or ‘hyperactive’ or just those considered “bright underachievers”. Easily administered tests to help identify individuals who may be at risk are discussed. When RPS occurs without the necessary sensory-motor accommodation or compensation, we theorize that written language disability (WLD), as well as compromised organizational skills often identified as ADHD, result. In addition, this paper discusses a summary of our findings examining families with biologically related family members reporting primary difficulties related to organization and written language skills. Case studies from four of the many families we have studied demonstrate how to determine if an individual is able to sense his/her own body extremities in correctly functioning or maladaptive directional orientation. In some cases, just changing a writing hand position has been shown to help overcome many of the problems of RPS. This change is meant is to redirect the writing hand movement to sense a top/down orientation.

Share and Cite:

Young, R. , Ginsburg, B. & Bradway, D. (2012). Physical and Behavioral Markers Help Identify Written Language Disability (WLD) Related to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Psychology, 3, 36-44. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.31006.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Adams, J., Gopher, D., & Lintern, G. (1977). Effects of visual and proprioceptive feedback on motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 9, 11-22.
[2] Bradway, D. A. (2003). Behavioral genetic investigation into directional motor skills and reading disability. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs.
[3] Carter, B. F. (1977). Patterns of cerebral asymmetry in families affected by specific developmental dyslexia. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs.
[4] Carter, B. F., Ginsburg, B. E., & Isaacs, S. (1982). Right-handed families: A genealogical rarity. Behavior Genetics, 23, 579.
[5] Cook, A. S., & Woollacot, M. H. (2006). Motor control: Translating research into clinical practice. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
[6] Educational Records Bureau (1982). The Educational Records Bureau Aptitude and Achievement Test. Level 4-Form C. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
[7] Ginsburg, B. E., Carter, B. F., & Sadick, T. L. (1980). Patterns of laterality in children: Sinister effects of sinistrality in males. Behavior Genetics, 10, 494.
[8] Ginsburg, B. E., Carter, B. F., & Sadick, T. L. (1981). A genetic model of human laterality pattern. Behavior Genetics, 11, 600.
[9] The Riverside Publishing Company, & Houghton Mifflin Company (1993). Iowa Tests of Basic Skills.
[10] Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., et al. (2009). The forgotten learning disability: Epidemiology of written language disorder in a population-based birth cohort (1976-1982), Rochester, Minnesota. Pediatrics, 123, 1306-1313. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-2098
[11] Haager, D. (2007). Promises and cautions regarding using response to intervention with English language learners. Learning Disability Quarterly, 30, 213-218. doi:10.2307/30035565
[12] LeDoux, J. E., Wilson, D. H., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1977). Manipulo- spatial aspects of cerebral lateralization Clues to the origin of lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 14, 743-750. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(77)90004-5
[13] Longstaff, J. A. (2003). Model for practical kinesthesia. The Conference of the International Association for Dance Medicine & Science (IADMS), London, UK, October.
[14] Lyon, G. R. (1996). Learning disabilities. The Future of Children, 6, 54-76. doi:10.2307/1602494
[15] Robertson, S. D. (2001). Development of bimanual skill: The search for stable patterns of coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 114- 126. doi:10.1080/00222890109603144
[16] Sadick, T. L., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1978). The development of the lateral functions and reading ability. Cortex., 14, 3-11.
[17] Young, R. A., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1987, 2011). Young-Ginsburg lateral direction assessment and spatial test; “YGLD” Storrs, CT.
[18] Young, R. A., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1987). Genetic variations in motor and cognitive patterns associated with reading disabilities—diagnosis and remediation. Behavior Genetics, 17, 644.
[19] Young, R. A. (1989). Preferred directional rotational motor movement of the fingers and hands compared to reported receptive/expressive written language ability. Unpublished Thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs.
[20] Young, R. A., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1993). The directional motor link in reading disabilities; in temporal information processing in the nervous system—With special reference to dyslexia and dysphasia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 436-439. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb23016.x
[21] Young, R. A., & Ginsburg, B. E. (1995). Visuo-motor patterns in dyslexia. Behavior Genetics, 25, 294.
[22] Young, R. A. (2004). Laterality in kinesthetic rotation of movement orientation and visual dominance interaction as mediators of con- sciousness; Toward a science of consciousness. The Consciousness Conference at the University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, April.
[23] Young, R. A. (2006). Tested lateral visual and motor behavior inter- action identify suspected aspects of Learning Disabilities. Behavior Genetics, 36, 952-990.
[24] Young, R. A. (2008). Lost in Space-“Inverted Positioning Sensation” (IPS): as of yet unidentified contributing cause for ADHD and/or LD. The Consciousness Conference at the University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, Abstract 121.
[25] Wender, P. H., Wolf, L. E., & Wasserstein, J. (2001) Adults with ADHD: An overview. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 1-16. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05770.x
[26] Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, ed. (WISC-II) (1986).
[27] Wolf, P. H., Melngallis, I., Obreon, M., & Bedrosian, M. (1996) Family patterns of developmental dyslexia part III: Spelling errors as behavioral phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 67, 378-386. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960726)67:4<378::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-G

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.