Exploring Gender Conformity in a South African Tertiary Institution: A Field Experiment

Abstract

This field experiment aimed to answer the following research question: Is there a significant association between gender and conformity in a South African context? The methodological foundations for our research question (within a South African context) were laid by the United States of America field experiment of Carter et al. (2019) and accordingly unobtrusive observation was used to observe male and female conformity. The setting for the field experiment was a cafeteria on campus at a tertiary institution in South Africa. Statistical data analysis revealed a significant association between gender and conformity, with males less conforming and more non-conforming than expected, and females more conforming and less non-conforming than expected. Moreover, a content analysis of observed verbal behavior revealed the following five categories: females expressed anger, females critically questioned gender signs, males expressed amusement at the experience, males casually inquired about gender signs, and finally that groups influence vocal expression. In contrast to international studies, our results show high rates of non-conformity and amusement amongst males and high rates of conformity and anger amongst females.

Share and Cite:

du Toit, R. , de Klerk, W. , Barnard, M. , Botha, J. , Matlou, T. , van der Merwe, M. , Visagie, A. , Botha, K. , Spies, R. and Opperman, I. (2022) Exploring Gender Conformity in a South African Tertiary Institution: A Field Experiment. Psychology, 13, 1823-1834. doi: 10.4236/psych.2022.1313113.

1. Introduction

Schroeder and Liben (2021) noted that individuals are exposed to messages from their families, peers, communities, and the media that perpetuate traditional gender roles. Consequently, expectations about how males and females should behave or express gender are generally structured by facets such as cultures or societies (Mantell et al., 2009) parents’ attitudes (Jackson et al., 2021; Lan & Isacoff, 2022) or even the self and other peers (Jackson et al., 2021). Although these roles are dynamic in how they are expressed over time, males and females still accept these present relations as natural and fixed and continuously refer to the economic, social and cultural roles, behaviors and attributes that are associated with being male or female (Zhu & Chang, 2019). For the purpose of this current field experiment male and female identification was based on physiological characteristics evaluated by the observers (see Lindqvist et al., 2021). The researchers acknowledge that an individual’s gender identification cannot be exclusively tied to sexual dimorphism as binary gender systems do not accurately reflect subjective lived experiences for individuals. However, in South Africa, like in other countries, gender discrimination and inequality break down along the lines of a binary gender system where women and men often need to conform to gender norms relating to uneven gender-specific experiences (Casale & Posel, 2021).

1.1. Gender Norms

Gender norms and expectations begin in the earliest stages of an individual’s life and are crucial in shaping a person’s attitudes, opportunities, experiences, and behaviors (Weber et al., 2019). Jackson et al. (2021) capture this as gender typicality, thus, how individuals understand their interests, personality, and behaviors that should align with stereotypical characteristics attributed to other males and females. Therefore, gender norms can be regarded as the spoken and unspoken societal rules imposed on individuals (Morgenroth & Ryan, 2018; Weber et al., 2019). These rules dictate the accepted and expected societal behaviors of the individual according to their biological sex (Weber et al., 2019). Pearse and Connel (2016) describe this as not just the attitudes in individuals’ minds but embedded within the broader structures, systems, and institutions of society.

Within contemporary societies, Pearse and Connell (2016) argue that norms that act as hindrances to gender equality are not the only gender norms to consider, but the inverse also exists. The symbolic dimension in which we define gender norms is a significant element in the existence of a gendered social life, that can be found in all forms of social practices within all institutions (Pearse & Connell, 2016).

1.2. Problem Statement

Carter et al. (2019) conducted an experiment (consisting of two studies) in the United States of America to examine the influences of gender norms on gender-role conformity. Through conducting an unobtrusive observation, the researchers observed and recorded the conformity rates of males and females, after placing gender signs on doors at the entrance of a building. The observations found in their study that males tend to conform more to the gender role signs than females (Carter et al., 2019).

Carter et al. (2019) recommended that future research should be conducted in a more diverse population. As far as the researchers’ knowledge, a similar field experiment to that of Carter et al. (2019) has not been conducted in South Africa. Therefore, the purpose of this (our) field experiment was to conceptually investigate the influences of gender norms on gender-role conformity in South Africa. The study of Carter et al. (2019) laid the methodological foundation from which our research question was drawn and from which observations were conducted. From the above-mentioned experiment, we posed the following research question: Is there a significant association between gender and conformity in a South African context?

2. Method

2.1. Observation Methodology and Setting

This field experiment utilized unobtrusive observation to record the gender conformity rates of males and females within the observation setting. In this field experiment (Kassin et al., 2014), researchers assumed the “complete observer” role which enabled the researchers (observers) to go unnoticed and unseen (Nieuwenhuis, 2016). The setting for the observation was a cafeteria on campus at a tertiary institution in South Africa. The population of the tertiary institution includes undergraduate and postgraduate students (N = 53,071). The population gender includes 59% females and 41% males with diverse racial backgrounds: Black African (68.8%), White (26.4%), Coloured (3.57%), Asian (1.0%) and other (0.09%). The cafeteria was identified as the ideal setting as it had two turnstile entrances, it had the potential to produce a large number of individuals to observe, and the researchers were more likely to go unnoticed. Two custom signs with a size of 210 mm × 297 mm each were displayed at a height of 140 mm on the separate turnstiles. The first sign was a male-only sign with a male symbol, an arrow to the right, and the words “male only” written underneath. The second sign was a female-only sign with a female symbol, an arrow to the right, and the words “female only” written underneath. See Figure 1 for sign placement.

Unstructured observations with the researchers as a complete observer were conducted for 60 minutes. Before the commencement of the field experiment, it was noticed that a pillar near the turnstiles had the potential to obstruct the long-range view of the sign on the right turnstile. Therefore, the “male only” and “female only” signs were switched after 30 minutes. During the first 10 minutes of the observation one observer (researcher 1) took notes of comments individuals made as they entered the turnstiles, two observers (researchers 2 and 3) observed male conformity, and two observers (researchers 4 and 5) observed female conformity. See Figure 2 for the field experiment layout for the initial 10 minutes.

Figure 1. Turnstile sign placement (after 30 minutes the male and female signs were switched around).

Figure 2. Cafeteria entrance layout and planned observer (researcher) placement.

After 10 minutes the observer (researcher 1) at the food order counter requested that a second observer (researcher 5) join them as they were not able to understand all the comments being made due to language barriers. Thereafter, two observers (researchers 1 and 5) noted the comments of individuals as they passed through the turnstiles and three observers (researchers 2, 3 and 4) observed male and female conformity and non-conformity. See Figure 3 for the

Figure 3. Changes made to observer (researcher) placement after 10 minutes.

changes in researcher (observer) positions.

2.2. Methodological Rigor

The field notes (anecdotal records) taken during the field experiment included comments made by individuals in the area, the context of the field experiment, deviations from the planned methodology, experiences of the observers (researchers), and conforming and non-conforming males and females. The data gathered in the field notes were used to inform the meaning of observed participants’ verbal behavior reactions (Maharaj, 2016). Moreover, the field notes served as a critical reflection tool as the observers (researchers) discussed their experiences, thoughts, and impressions with two senior researchers immediately upon completion of the field experiment (Johnson et al., 2020; Maharaj, 2016). Inter-coder testing was applied during the content analysis to increase the transparency of the coding process of observed verbal behavior made by participants and to reduce the possibility of researcher (observer) bias (O’Connor & Joffe, 2020). Confirmability and transparency of the field experiment are ensured by providing a rich description of the methodology and context (Schurink et al., 2021). This detailed description can provide future researchers with the necessary information to decide if the results are relevant to their purposes (Schurink et al., 2021).

3. Results

3.1. Statistical Analysis

Table 1 and Figure 4 depict the results of the observational study field experiment, which included (N = 167) individuals. Of the observed individuals, 92 (55.1%) were male and 75 (44.9%) were female.

The majority of individuals (62.8%) conformed to the gender signs and the data indicated that more females (72%) conformed than males (55.4%).

A chi-square test (χ2) indicated that there exists a significant association between gender and conformity, [χ2 (1, N = 167) = 4.71, p = 0.041]. According to the observed and expected values, males were less conforming and more non-conforming than expected, in contrast, females were more conforming and less non-conforming than expected.

3.2. Qualitative Data Analysis

Content analysis (Hsieh & Shannon, 2005) of the observed verbal behavior yielded five core categories; two for females, two for males, and one for groups

Table 1. Conforming and non-conforming males and females.

Figure 4. Conformity and non-conformity graph.

(see Table 2). Evidence is presented for each category (with frequencies) and relevant discussion thereof.

Category 1: Females Expressed Anger

Females expressed anger after entering turnstiles. Observed verbal behavior was as follows: “This f% is sexist”; “Sexist, not inclusive”; and “F% norms”. According to the literature, conforming to restrictive gender norms has a greater negative impact on female mental health than on males (Sanchez et al., 2005). Should females have perceived the signs as discriminatory, they may consequently have expressed anger towards them. Additionally, because females have previously been disadvantaged, they are more sensitive to gender discrimination than males (Drury & Kalser, 2014; Khan et al., 2018).

Category 2: Females Critically Questioned Gender Signs

Females critically questioned why gender signs were required in the social setting. Observed verbal behavior was as follows: “What the hell?”; “Why is this thing discriminating?”; and “What if someone doesnt identify with a gender?”. Literature indicates that females are more sensitive to sexism than males are (Drury & Kalser, 2014; Khan et al., 2018), therefore if they perceived the signs as sexist, they may have consequently questioned them.

Category 3: Males Expressed Amusement at the Experience

Males expressed amusement, laughing, and intentionally not conforming to gender signs. Observed verbal behavior was as follows: “I dont know [male participant laughing]”; “Two males entering the female-gendered turnstile and laughing”; and “Five male participants laughing as they entered the turnstiles”. Previous research indicates that males may use humor that trivializes females as a way to improve in-group cohesion (Meghana & Vijaya, 2020; Thomae & Pine, 2015). Males in groups may have expressed amusement and non-conformity to improve the social cohesion of their group.

Category 4: Males Casually Inquired about Gender Signs

Males questioned the gender signs in a light-hearted manner; their questions were not as critical as those of females. Observed verbal behavior was as follows: “I wonder who put this up?” and “What is happening here [male participant laughing]?” Males, according to Drury and Kalser (2014), are less likely than females to identify sexism and thus less likely to take a sexist situation seriously. As a result, males may not have expressed strong opinions about sexism or discrimination such as those of females if they did not recognize the situation in

Table 2. Categories of the experiences of individuals after entering turnstiles.

that way.

Category 5: Groups Influence Vocal Expression

In groups of the same gender, individuals were more vocally expressive about the signs. In groups of mixed gender, individuals were less vocally expressive. Observed verbal behavior was as follows: “Female entered with a male, made no comment” and “Group of males entered and loudly laughed”. Research suggests males may perceive females negatively when females confront gender signs (Anisman-Razin et al., 2018), consequently females may not have made critical comments when with a male. Additionally, individuals had similar expressions in groups of the same gender, possibly due to the effect of group opinion on individual behavior (Klucharev et al., 2009).

4. Discussion

This field experiment aimed to investigate if there is a significant association between gender and conformity within a South African context. Although studies in the United States of America explored the effects of gender norms on gender role conformity, no such study (to our knowledge) has been conducted with a diverse population such as that of South Africa. Our field experiment provided data on the number of conforming and non-conforming males and females, as well as qualitative data (observed verbal behavior) from field notes (anecdotal records). The qualitative data provided categories to complement the statistical data gathered.

One of the key results, that more individuals conformed, is consistent with Carter et al.’s (2019) study of gender conformity. According to Carter et al. (2019), individuals are more likely to conform to the norms set out by their in-group to avoid rejection, ridicule, or harsh criticism.

In contrast to Carter et al.’s (2019) study, higher rates of non-conformity were observed among males in our field experiment. There has been little research to help explain this result, however, the qualitative data of our field experiment may provide insight. Content analysis revealed that males expressed amusement at the experience. Male students’ humor and intentional disobedience may contribute to group social approval and improved group cohesion (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Meghana & Vijaya, 2020; Thomae & Pine, 2015). If male students in this setting entered the turnstiles in groups more frequently, the influence of such groups may have contributed to the high rates of non-conformity observed.

An additional result was that when we only looked at the results of females, they conformed to the gender signs on the turnstiles more than males. This supports the results of Della Vigna et al.’s (2012) study which showed that females are more likely to conform in public settings. Interestingly, in our field experiment females expressed anger while conforming to the gender signs. This contradicts research which indicates that in general men are more likely to express verbal anger (Fischer & Evers, 2011). Furthermore, when individuals are unsure about the correctness of their decisions, they are more likely to conform (Drače & Efendić, 2020). If females were unsure whether non-conformity was acceptable, they may have continued to conform despite expressing anger.

Finally, we found that groups of the same or opposite gender influenced individuals’ vocal expressiveness. Individuals, male and female, expressed similar opinions when grouped with others of the same gender. Groups of females, in particular, loudly expressed anger and dissatisfaction with the gender signs, whereas when paired with a male or in a group with males, they did not appear to express the same anger. Females may have reserved such opinions around males to avoid potential negative perceptions (Anisman-Razin et al., 2018).

In contrast to international studies, our results show high rates of non-conformity and amusement amongst males and high rates of conformity and anger amongst females. Within the South African context, it is believed that all racial and ethnic groups have long-standing gender roles, which are based on the fundamentals that women are less important or deserving of power (Mudau & Obadire, 2017). This consequently adds to the notion that most African traditional social systems are centered and dominated by males. Additionally, individual expressiveness appears to be determined by group influence, which may also have influenced whether or not individuals conformed to the gender signs.

5. Limitations and Recommendations

This field experiment was not without limitations. Language barriers experienced during the first 10 minutes of the observation could have resulted in relevant data not being recorded. Observational data will not always be objective as the researcher as a research instrument is subjective which can influence the information observed and analyzed (Strydom, 2021). Furthermore, not all individuals entering the cafeteria may have noted the signs once they were placed. The results of this field experiment therefore cannot be generalized to other populations and should be interpreted with caution. Furthermore, the display of male and female-only signs may have negatively impacted individuals as well as the observed results, as not all individuals relate to the assigned gender operational definition within this field experiment (see Lindqvist et al., 2021).

It is recommended that future research should include a larger sample size in multiple contexts within South Africa. Furthermore, as culture is argued to play a role in the meaning of conformity and non-conformity (Hiller & Baudin, 2016), future research may explore South African individuals’ ethnic influences on gender conformity.

6. Ethical Considerations

Per the South Africa Department of Health (2015), ethical clearance for observing people in public spaces need not go through formal ethic clearance. The researchers did not interact directly with individuals, no direct intervention was staged, and individuals or groups’ privacy and identity were not impeded. Permission to conduct the field experiment was obtained from the tertiary institutional cafeteria staff.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the North-West University Master of Health Sciences in Research Psychology Programme students of 2022 (Megan Barnard, Jessica Botha, Tebogo Matlou, Melissa van der Merwe and Arina Visagie), who conducted the field experiment. We also want to thank Miss Elinda Harmse (independent Research Psychologist) who acted as the peer reviewer for this written manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Anisman-Razin, M., Kark, R., & Saguy, T. (2018). “Putting Gender on the Table”: Understanding Reactions to Women Who Discuss Gender Inequality. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 21, 690-706. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430217744648
[2] Carter, M. F., Franz, T. M., Gruschow, J. L., & Van Ryne, A. M. (2019). The Gender Conformity Conundrum: The Effects of Irrelevant Gender Norms on Public Conformity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159, 761-765.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2019.1586636
[3] Casale, D., & Posel, D. (2021). Gender Inequality and the COVID-19 Crisis: Evidence from a Large National Survey during South Africa’s Lockdown. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 71, Article ID: 100569.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2020.100569
[4] Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social Influence: Compliance and Conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591-621.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015
[5] Della Vigna, S., List, J. A., Malmendier, U., & Rao, G. (2012). The Importance of Being Marginal: Gender Differences in Generosity. American Economic Review, 103, 586-590.
https://www.aeaweb.org/articles?id=10.1257/aer.103.3.586
https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.103.3.586
[6] Department of Health (2015). Ethics in Health Research: Principles, Processes and Structures.
https://www.ul.ac.za/research/application/downloads/DoH%202015%20Ethics%20in%20Health%20Research%20Guidelines.pdf
[7] Drače, S., & Efendić, E. (2020). The Effect of Specific Emotions on Conformity. Psihologija, 55, 45-56. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI200109038D
[8] Drury, B. J., & Kaiser, C. R. (2014). Allies against Sexism: The Role of Men in Confronting Sexism. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 637-652. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12083
[9] Fischer, A. H., & Evers, C. (2011). The Social Costs and Benefits of Anger as a Function of Gender and Relationship Context. Sex Roles, 65, 23-34.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9956-x
[10] Hiller, V., & Baudin, T. (2016). Cultural Transmission and the Evolution of Gender Roles. Mathematical Social Sciences, 84, 8-23.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mathsocsci.2016.08.002
[11] Hsieh, J.-F., & Shannon, S. E. (2005). Three Approaches to Qualitative Content Analysis. Qualitative Health Research, 15, 1277-1288. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732305276687
[12] Jackson, E. F., Bussey, K., & Myers, E. (2021). Encouraging Gender Conformity or Sanctioning Nonconformity? Felt Pressure from Parents, Peers, and the Self. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50, 613-627. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01387-8
[13] Johnson, J. L., Adkins, D., & Chauvin, S. (2020). Qualitative Research in Pharmacy Education: A Review of the Quality Indicators of Rigor in Qualitative Research. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 84, Article 7120.
https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe7120
https://www.ajpe.org/content/ajpe/84/3/ajpe7129.full.pdf
[14] Kassin, S., Fein, S., & Markus, H. R. (2014). Social Psychology (9th ed.). Cengage Learning.
[15] Khan, M. J., Arooj, K., Arif, H., Nazir, N., & Nosheen, M. (2018). Attitude of Male and Female University Students towards Gender Discrimination. Pakistan Journal of Psychological Research, 33, 429-436.
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Mussarat-Khan/publication/329888265_Attitude_of_male_and_female_university_students_towards_gender_discrimination/links/5c7f711892851c695058d46d/Attitude-of-male-and-female-university-students-towards-gender-discrimination.pdf
[16] Klucharev, V., Hytönen, K., Rijpkema, M., Smidts, A., & Fernández, G. (2009). Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity. Neuron, 61, 140-151.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027
[17] Lan, M., & Isacoff, N. (2022). Parents’ Attitudes Predict Adolescent Gender Expression. Pschology, 13, 301-312. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2022.132018
[18] Lindqvist, A., Sendén, M. G., & Renström, E. A. (2021). What Is Gender, Anyway: A Review of the Options for Operationalising Gender. Psychology & Sexuality, 12, 332-344.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19419899.2020.1729844
[19] Maharaj, N. (2016). Using Field Notes to Facilitate Critical Reflection. Reflective Practice, 17, 114-124. https://doi.org/10.1080/14623943.2015.1134472
[20] Mantell, J. E., Needham, S. L., Smit, J. A., Hoffman, S., Cebekhulu, Q., Adams-Skinner, J., Exner, T. M., Mabude, Z., Beksiinska, M., Stein, Z. A., & Milford, C. (2009). Gender Norms in South Africa: Implications for HIV and Pregnancy Prevention among African and Indian Women Students at a South African Tertiary Institution. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 11, 139-157. https://doi.org/10.1080/14623943.2015.1134472
[21] Meghana, J., & Vijaya, R. (2020). Humour and Gender Stereotypes. IASSI Quarterly, 39, 58-75. https://www.academia.edu/43967396/Humour_and_Gender_Stereotypes
[22] Morgenroth, T., & Ryan, M. K. (2018). Gender Trouble in Social Psychology: How can Butler’s Work Inform Experimental Social Psychologists’ Conceptualization of Gender? Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1320. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01320
[23] Mudau, T. J., & Obadire, O. S. (2017). The Role of Patriarchy in Family Settings and Its Implications to Girls and Women in South Africa. Journal of Human Ecology, 58, 67-72. https://doi.org/10.1080/09709274.2017.1305614
[24] Nieuwenhuis, J. (2016). Qualitative Research Designs and Data-Gathering techniques. In K. Maree (Ed.), First Steps in Research (2nd ed., pp. 72-103). Van Schaik Publishers.
[25] O’Connor, C., & Joffe, H. (2020). Intercoder Reliability in Qualitative Research: Debates and Practical Guidelines. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 19.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1609406919899220
[26] Pearse, R., & Connell, R. (2016). Gender Norms and the Economy: Insights from Social Research. Feminist Economics, 22, 30-53.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13545701.2015.1078485
[27] Sanchez, D. T., Crocker, J., & Boike, K. R. (2005). Doing Gender in the Bedroom: Investing in Gender Norms and the Sexual Experience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1445-1455. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205277333
[28] Schroeder, K. M., & Liben, L. S. (2021). Felt Pressure to Conform to Cultural Gender Roles: Correlates and Consequences. Sex Roles, 84, 125-138.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01155-9
[29] Schurink, W. J., Schurink, E. M., & Fouche, C. B. (2021). Qualitative Data Analysis and Interpretation. In C. B. Fouche, H. Strydom, & W. J. H. Roestenburg (Eds.), Research at Grass Roots (5th ed., pp. 391-416). Van Schaik Publishers.
[30] Strydom, H. (2021). Contemporary Data Collection Methods. In C. B. Fouche, H. Strydom, & W. J. H. Roestenburg (Eds.), Research at Grass Roots (5th ed., pp. 335-354). Van Schaik Publishers.
[31] Thomae, M., & Pina, A. (2015). Sexist Humor and Social Identity: The Role of Sexist Humor in Men’s in-Group Cohesion, Sexual Harassment, Rape Proclivity, and Victim Blame. HUMOR, 28, 187-204. https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2015-0023
[32] Weber, A. M., Cislaghi, B., Meausoone, V., Abdalla, S., Mejía-Guevara, I., Loftus, P., Hallgren, E., Seff, I., Stark, L., Victora, C. G., Buffarini, R., Barros, A. J. D., Domingue, B. W., Bhushan, D., Gupta, R., Nagata, J. M., Skakya, H. B., Richet, L. M., Norris, S. A. et al. (2019). Gender Norms and Health: Insights from Global Survey Data. The Lancet, 393, 2455-2468. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(19)30765-2
[33] Zhu, N., & Chang, L. (2019). Evolved but Not Fixed: A Life History Account of Gender roles and Gender Inequality. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1709.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01709

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.