Left-Right and Up-Down Mirror Image Confusion in 4-, 5- and 6-Year-Olds

Abstract

Young children under the age of 8 - 9 years tend to confuse left-right mirror images, and it is thought that their linguistic skills play a crucial role in this phenomenon. However, other aspects of this confusion, such as whether children confuse up-down mirror images or whether the meaningfulness of the stimulus influences matching performance, remain unclear. The present study examined the confusion of left-right and up-down reversed images by 4-, 5- and 6-year-olds using meaningful and meaningless figures in a task in which sample and comparison stimuli were presented simultaneously. Children performed more accurately when presented with meaningful figures and confused both up-down and left-right reversed figures, although they did so less frequently in response to up-down than to left-right reversed figures. Reversal confusion was greatest in 4-year-olds and no significant differences were observed between 5- and 6-year-olds. These findings suggest that the ability to discriminate reversed images may be associated with the development of a wide range of cognitive abilities including theory of mind, executive function, and suggestibility.

Share and Cite:

Uehara, I. (2013). Left-Right and Up-Down Mirror Image Confusion in 4-, 5- and 6-Year-Olds. Psychology, 4, 736-740. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.410104.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419-439.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.419
[2] Bryant, P. E. (1973). Discrimination of mirror images by young children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 415-425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034141
[3] Cohn, M., & Stricker, G. (1979). Reversal errors in strong, average, and weak letter namers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 533-537.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221947901200808
[4] Corballis, M. C., & Zalk, M. C. (1977). Why do children confuse mirror-image obliques? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 516-523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(77)90095-9
[5] Cronin, V. (1967). Mirror-image reversal discrimination in kindergarten and first-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 5, 577-585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(67)90051-3
[6] Cubelli, R., & Della Sala, S. (2009). Mirror writing in pre-school children: A pilot study. Cognitive Processing, 10, 101-104.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0233-z
[7] Davidson, H. P. (1935). A study of the confusing letters b, d, p, and q. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 47, 458-468.
[8] Fischer, J. P., & Tazouti, Y. (2012). Unraveling the mystery of mirror writing in typically developing children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 193-205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025735
[9] Fisher, C. B., Bornstein, M. H., & Gross, C. G. (1985). Left-right coding and skills related to beginning reading. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 6, 279-283.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-198510000-00009
[10] Flavell, J., Green, F., & Flavell, E. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, i+iii+v+1-87.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1165866
[11] Gerstadt, C., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2 - 7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129-153.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01105.x
[12] Huttenlocher, J. (1967). Discrimination of figure orientation: Effects of relative position. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 359-361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0024398
[13] Jordan, B. T., & Jordan, S. G. (1974). Jordan left-right reversal test. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 4, 178-187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01436026
[14] Jordan, B. T., & Jordan, S. G. (1990). Jordan left-right reversal test: An analysis of visual reversals in children and significance for reading problems. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 21, 65-73.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00709929
[15] Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Developmental Psychology, 31, 568-578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.31.5.758
[16] Lyons, K. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Monitoring, metacognition, and executive function: Elucidating the role of self-reflection in the development of self-regulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 40, 379-412.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386491-8.00010-4
[17] Mandler, J. M., & Stein, N. L. (1974). Recall and recognition of pictures by children as a function of organization and distractor similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 657-669.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036114
[18] McMonnies, C. W. (1992a). Reversals and left/right confusion. Journal of Behavioral Optometry, 3, 31-34.
[19] McMonnies, C. W. (1992b). Visuo-spatial discrimination and mirror image letter reversals in reading. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 63, 698-704.
[20] Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Gornik, E. (2007). Episodic memory development: Theory of mind is part of re-experiencing experienced events. Infant and Child Development, 16, 471-490.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.517
[21] Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of child amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516-548.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1995.1024
[22] Povinelli D. J., Landau, K. R., & Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 1540-1554.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01813.x
[23] Rudel, R. G., & Teuber, H. L. (1963). Discrimination of direction of line in children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 892-898. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046592
[24] Sapp, F., Lee, K., & Muir, D. (2000). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with the appearance-reality distinction: Is it real or is it apparent? Developmental Psychology, 36, 547-560.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.36.5.547
[25] Terepocki, M., Kruk, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (2002). The incidence and nature of letter orientation errors in reading disability. Journal of Learning and disabilities, 35, 214-233.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500304
[26] Thompson, G. B. (1975). Discrimination of mirror-image shapes by young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 165-176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(75)90157-5
[27] Uehara, I. (1998). No transfer of visuomotor learning of button-pressing from right to left hands in right-handed four-year-olds. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 87, 1427-1440.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.3f.1427
[28] Uehara I. (2000). Differences in episodic memory between four- and five-year-olds: False information versus real experiences. Psychological Reports, 86, 745-755.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3.745
[29] Uehara, I. (March, 2012). Differentiation of left-right reversal figures and letters by young children. The 23rd Annual Meeting for of the Japan Society of Developmental Psychology, Nagoya. (in Japanese)
[30] Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 12-17.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001
[31] Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development of executive function in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 121-125.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10774761

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.