When Children Draw vs When Children Don’t: Exploring the Effects of Observational Drawing in Science

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate how kindergarten children’s observational drawings impact their information retention. This research was conducted in an urban school in a large metropolitan area in the southwestern United States. Forty-two kindergarten children participated in this study; approximately 97% of them qualified for free and/or reduced lunch. For this study, children’s retention of factual information was compared using a paired t-test of when they drew and when they didn’t. Children scored higher on all 7 items—descriptions of observation, location, action, color, size, shape, and sound—when they drew than when they didn’t. Findings were statistically significant for descriptions of observation (t = 3.08, p = .00) and location (t = 2.36, p = .02).

Share and Cite:

Fox, J. & Lee, J. (2013). When Children Draw vs When Children Don’t: Exploring the Effects of Observational Drawing in Science. Creative Education, 4, 11-14. doi: 10.4236/ce.2013.47A1002.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Abruscato, J. (2004). Teaching children science. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
[2] Alden, A. (2009). What is observing? An explanation of observation. http://geology.about.com/od/
activitiesbasics/a/observation.htm
[3] American Association for the Advancement of Science (1997). Resources for scientific literacy: Professional development. New York: Oxford University Press.
[4] Bosse, S., Jacobs, G., & Anderson, T. L. (2009). Science in the air. Young Children, 64, 10-15.
[5] Brooks, M. (2003). Drawing to learn. NAEYC Beyond Journal. http://www.naeyc.org/files/yc/file/2003
09/DrawingtoLearn.pdf
[6] Brooks, M. (2009). Drawing, visualization and young children’s exploration of “Big Ideas.” International Journal of Science Education, 31, 319-341. doi:10.1080/09500690802595771
[7] California Department of Education (1998). Science content standards for California public schools. http://www.cde.ca.gov/be/st/ss/documents/sciencestnd.pdf
[8] Connecticut State Department of Education (2004). Core science curriculum framework. http://www.sde.ct.gov/sde/cwp/view.asp?a=26188q=320890
[9] Dove, J. E., Everett, L. A., & Preece, P. F. W. (1999). Exploring a hydrological concept through children’s drawings. International Journal of Science Education, 21, 485-497. doi:10.1080/095006999290534
[10] Eberbach, C., & Crowley, K. (2009). From every day to scientific observation: How children learn to observe the biologist’s world. Re view of Educational Research, 79, 39-68. doi:10.3102/0034654308325899
[11] Edwards, C., Gandini, L., & Forman, G. (1998). The hundred languages of children: The Reggio Emilia approach advanced reflections (2nd ed.). Westport, CT: Ablex.
[12] Ford, D. (2005). The challenges of observing geologically: Third graders’ descriptions of rock and mineral properties. International Journal of Science, 89, 276-295.
[13] Fox, J. E. (2010). The role of drawing in kindergarteners’ science observations. International Research Journal for Art in Early Childhood Education, 2.
[14] Howes, E. V. (2007). Educative experiences and early childhood science education: A Deweyan perspective on learning to observe. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24, 536-549. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2007.03.006
[15] Howitt, C., Lewis, S., & Upson, E. (2011). “It’s a mystery!” a case study of implementing forensic science in preschool as scientific inquiry. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 36, 45-55.
[16] Jolley, R. P. (2010). Children and pictures: Drawing and understanding. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.
[17] Kaatz, K. (2008). A walk in the “tall, tall grass.” Science and Children, 45, 28-31.
[18] Kallery, M., Psillos, D., & Tselfes, V. (2009). Typical didactical active ties in the Greek early years science classroom: Do they promote science learning? International Journal of Science Education, 31, 1187-1204. doi:10.1080/09500690701824850
[19] Kepler, L. (1998). Hands-on science: Journals of science. Instructor, 108, 82-83.
[20] National Committee on Science Education Standards (1996). National science education standards. Washington DC: The National Academies Press.
[21] Quigley, C., Pongsanon, K., & Akerson, V. L. (2010). If we teach them, they can learn: Young students views of nature of science aspects to early elementary students during an informal science education program. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 21, 887-907. doi:10.1007/s10972-009-9164-5
[22] Samarapungavan, A., Patrick, H., & Manticopoulos, P. (2011). What kindergarten children learn in inquiry-based science classrooms. Cognition and Instruction, 29, 416-470. doi:10.1080/07370008.2011.608027
[23] Schulte, C. M. (2011). Verbalization in children’s drawing performances: Toward a metaphorical continuum, of inscription, extension, and re-inscription. Studies in Art Education, 53, 20-34.
[24] Shephardson, D. P., & Britsch, S. J. (2001). The role of children’s journals in elementary science activities. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 43-69. doi:10.1002/1098-2736(200101)38:1<43::AID-TEA4>3.0.CO;2-I
[25] Texas Education Agency (1998). Texas essential knowledge and skills. http://ritter.tea.state.tx.us/rules/tac/chapter112/index.html
[26] Virginia Department of Education (2009). Virginia standards of learning. http://www.doe.virginia.gov/testing/sol/standards_docs/science/2010/complete/stds_all_science.pdf

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.